Rollin M. Gallagher, MD, MPH, George Koob, PhD, and Adrian Popescu, MD
CHAPTER OUTLINE
■ CHRONIC PAIN
■ CLINICAL INTERFACE BETWEEN PAIN AND ADDICTION
■ CONCLUSIONS
Chronic pain is our society’s most common and most costly chronic illness (1,2). It has the potential to change a person’s life trajectory, sometimes catastrophically, when treated inadequately or unwisely. Too often, the desperation of patients with pain leads them to be particularly vulnerable to the promise of cure or relief from the marketing of new products and procedures that lack an evidence basis for effectiveness. Opioid analgesics pose a particular dilemma. With their incredible capacity to ease the suffering of pain, they strongly impact the reward systems in the brain that play a key role in the activation and perpetuation of addiction. Thus, the two together—chronic pain and addiction—present a “perfect storm” of complexity and a challenge to our health care system and to society at large. Patients, their families, their employers, their communities, and their health care providers share the burden of chronic pain, contribute to the perpetuation of unwise and ineffective policies and procedures, and have a role to play in improving the outcomes of chronic pain and the outcomes of addiction, particularly when they complicate each other. For purposes of this chapter, we will not be considering cancer pain caused by malignant cells that damage tissues and the nervous system; rather, we will be referring to the cluster of noncancer conditions and diseases associated with persistent pain (e.g., musculoskeletal conditions, neurologic damage) that share common neuropathophysiologic mechanisms, using the term “maldynia” (denoting “bad pain” or “pain as a disease”) as distinct from eudynia (normal, acute pain in response to threatened or actual tissue damage or injury). Unraveling the biopsychosocial factors that perpetuate maldynia and impact a person’s pain control and addressing these factors in an organized, prioritized, goal-oriented management plan challenge our understanding of behavioral neuroscience, our use of available medical and team-based skills, our capacity for empathy, and our patience and perseverance (3,4). This challenge extends to the optimization of our health care system so that it incentivizes outcomes instead of profit (5–7). The pathophysiology of maldynia does not alone account for its variable symptomatic and behavioral expression and psychosocial complexity. Neural and glial networks regulating pain perception and behavior interact with emotional regulation and reward systems influencing addiction. Physiologic responses in both are worthy of examination with the expectation that the coexistence of maldynia and addiction will lead to complex interactions, that is, addictive responses altered by maldynia and maldynia altered by addiction.
This chapter reviews the pathophysiology of maldynia and the theoretical and clinical evidence for its overlap with addiction. The mechanisms of pain perception and modulation in maldynia are reviewed, with discussion of those aspects likely to be impacted by the presence of addictive disease. In this regard, the role of central sensitization and kindling and affective responses including depression and anxiety in maldynia is discussed, particularly since these processes and comorbidities also play a role in the phenomenology and treatment of addiction. Finally, ideas about potential interactions between maldynia and addiction are suggested, focusing on how the general state of addiction, inherited characteristics of the individual, and neuroadaptations specific to opioid abuse might affect the coexpression of these phenomena.
CHRONIC PAIN
In contrast to our ability to prevent, minimize, and manage acute pain, the management of maldynia often presents a daunting challenge. First, the physiology of pain after initial onset becomes much more complex almost immediately— the longer the pain, the more complex the process. The concept of “chronification” increasingly supplants the older, less useful, dimensional division between acute and chronic pain (8,9). Pain is now more accurately described as a continuum of experience from the moment of its onset until its cessation, and that experience informs the next experience of pain, through the glial cellular and neuronal network memory systems. As Emily Dickenson so prophetically wrote 150 plus years ago,
Pain has an element of blank.
It knows not when it began,
Or if there was a day when it was not.
Its infinite realms contain its past,
Enlightened to perceive new periods of pain.
Chronic nociceptive pain, such as from arthritis, involves processes that include transduction/activation, transmission, modulation/augmentation, and perception, whereas chronic neuropathic pain, caused by damage to neural tissue, is often independent of transduction and involves persistent activation, transmission, modulation/augmentation, and perception. These processes will be discussed below in more detail. In both types of persistent pain, a number of biopsychosocial factors can potentially influence neuroplastic changes in the central nervous system (CNS) that will, in turn, determine the salience of the pain for a person, the impact of pain on their lives, and pain’s resistance to treatments that focus on the nociceptive stimulus (10–12). These factors include, but are not limited to, environmental context, meaning of pain, attention, mood and affect, anxiety, catastrophizing, and social and cognitive attention/reinforcement.
Although experiments using laboratory-induced nociception in animals can control for much genetic and experiential variability, this is not possible in humans suffering chronic or episodic pain. Every new episode or change in pain intensity, character, or localization activates neural and inflammatory mechanisms informed by prior nociceptive experience and cognitive–emotional processes that are influenced by current context and meaning conditioned by past experience. These processes involve inflammatory glial activating systems in the central nervous system interacting with neural networks subserved by a myriad of chemical messengers that communicate among sensory systems and various cognitive–emotional processing and behavioral systems (13,14).
Pain is always an intensely personal and unique experience. However, pain experiences do cluster in specific, predictable patterns that can be diagnostically informative, for example, the pattern of pain can be nearly diagnostic in such conditions as S1 radiculopathy, migraine, fibromyalgia, diabetic neuropathy, and postherpetic neuralgia. Clinicians caring for patients with maldynia face the challenge of formulating for each patient, to the degree possible in a particular clinical setting, the interaction of biopsychosocial factors and neurobehavioral processes that activate and perpetuate chronic pain. Then they must devise a feasible treatment plan that has the best chance of remediating the most salient factors to improve the longitudinal outcome (3,5).
Pain Anatomy and Physiology
Wall and Melzak’s (1965) (15) postulation of the gate theory of pain provided the first pathophysiologic model to coherently explain the phenomenology of pain perception and modulation that was observed clinically. One author’s (RG) first memorable clinical experience illustrating the “gating system” occurred when delivering babies in a rural family practice setting in the early 1970s. During training, he had witnessed childbirths in an urban city hospital, perceived by the mothers as culturally foreign and threatening, since often they had received little prenatal care. These women usually experienced severe pain, high anxiety, and almost always required systemic opioids and neural blockade during labor and delivery. In contrast, his rural patients, when provided prenatal care by their own family doctors in a community setting and supported by training in Lamaze techniques, would usually give birth without systemic analgesia, despite the same tissue injury and volleys of nociception inherent to the process. In many cases, at their preference, they delivered without regional blocks. This suggested that other factors such as psychological state and its physiologic effects somehow affected the transmission of pain sensation and its experiential severity. Other examples abound of the phenomenon of environmental conditions and psychological state altering pain experience following normally very painful nociceptive injury—such as injured athletes not experiencing pain during competition or wounded soldiers not experiencing pain during battle.
Like any theory explaining the phenomena of great salience, the gate theory initiated an explosion of pain science, inspiring a generation of clinical and basic scientists who have dramatically expanded our knowledge about the nature of pain physiology, pathology, and experience. A review of the number of pain-relevant articles retrieved in PubMed using keyword indicators “pain” and “nociception” reveals about a twofold increase in publications containing these terms in the title or abstract, with over 160,000 published in the last decade. This new science has radically changed our understanding of the pathophysiology of chronic pain conditions and has generated a plethora of new treatments targeted at different molecular and neurobehavioral mechanisms affecting pain sensation. Interest in clinical pain has similarly exploded. A similar search reveals dramatically increased penetration of the science of pain into the general medical literature. Clearly, pain science and pain management are no longer the province of just pain specialists and scientists; they have penetrated the collective consciousness of our clinical medical culture.
Naturally, this new knowledge about the complex mechanisms involved in the experience of pain has revealed multiple targets for treatments. Some aim to reduce the stimulus itself—for example, weight loss to reduce weight-bearing stress on injured or diseased joints in the lower limbs or lower spine or postural and ergonomics training in musculoskeletal diseases leading to less “transduction” (activation of nociceptors). Others aim to block the stimulus from reaching the spinal cord (transmission), for example, peripheral nerve blocks, or aim to deactivate the transmission of the pain signal in the spinal cord rostrally to the brain (transmission), for example, neuromodulation. And still others target central nervous system responses and pathologic neuroplasticity—for example, neural reprogramming using cognitive–behavioral and psychomotor trainings to improve coping and reverse pathologic neuroplastic changes (16–19). In the hopes of positively affecting all of these processes, clinicians commonly combine treatments, each targeting different mechanisms in the pathophysiologic matrix of maldynia (3,4,10,12,20–22).
This additive, integrated approach improves pain control and outcomes as demonstrated by meta-analytic studies of comprehensive rehabilitation center populations (23–26). Although scientific comparisons of a single proven treatment with combinations of two or more proven treatments are prohibitively expensive, multimodality treatment to improve functional outcomes, not just pain control, is relatively well established as the most effective and longest-lasting treatment for chronic non-cancer pain (CNCP).
The neurobiologic basis for Wall and Melzack’s functional gating system, now better understood, provides a conceptual framework for understanding the clinical presentation of pain and helps explain the often complementary effects of different treatments for pain. For convenience, the very complex phenomenology of pain can be characterized as three major “stages” involving different mechanisms depending on the nature and time course of the originating stimulus (27,28). The overall anatomy of the systems for pain perception and transmission is shown in Figure 93-1. The stages are (a) nociception, the consequences of a brief noxious stimulus; (b) persistent and/or repeated nociception, the consequences of a prolonged or repetitive noxious stimulus leading to tissue damage and peripheral inflammation; and (c) the consequences of neurologic damage, including peripheral neuropathies and peripheral and central sensitization. These stages are not mutually exclusive and overlap temporally and physiologically through the complex interaction among various brain regions and ascending and descending messages between the CNS and the body. Their end result is chronic pain perpetuated by one or more of several mechanisms: chronic inflammation or recurrent injury peripherally; ectopic excitability of peripheral pain neurons; excitation and priming of spinal microglial cells; structural reorganization (e.g., neuroplasticity) of neural networks in the periphery and central nervous system so that touch, movement, temperature, and emotional changes cause the sensation of pain (discussed later—see Table 93-1); and loss of descending inhibitory controls. At any given time, one or a combination of these pathophysiologic mechanisms may be contributing to the experience of maldynia. Figures 93-2 and 93-3 pictorially represents this cycle and recycle of neurophysiologic events and biopsychosocial phenomenology leading to pain’s sometimes terrible consequences that are captured by the construct of maldynia.
FIGURE 93-1 Anatomy of pain perception.
FIGURE 93-2 Key: After injury to first-order nociceptive fibers, they spontaneously and repetitively discharge, activating the biochemical cascade, leading to central sensitization and neuroplasticity. Pain activates the stress response, including the sympathetic system, which can also be activated by emotional states such as anxiety or frustration not directly related to pain. Sympathetic terminals on injured nerves and/or nociceptors release norepinephrine, respectively, increasing ectopic discharge of injured nerves and/or lowering nociceptor firing threshold in injured tissue. This pathophysiologic process increases pain such that commonly patients with maldynia are known to “hole up” socially, avoiding interpersonal contact and other situations that might aggravate them and worsen their pain. (Adapted from Woolf CJ, Mannion RJ. Neuropathic pain: aetiology, symptoms, mechanisms, and management. Lancet1999;353(9168):1959–1964; Attal N, Bouhassira D. Mechanisms of pain in peripheral neuropathy. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl1999;173:12–24; discussion 48–52.
FIGURE 93-3 Acute to chronic pain cycle. (From Gallagher RM. Pharmacological approaches to pain management. In: Ebert M, Kerns R, eds. Behavioral and psychopharmacological pain management. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, 2010:129–141.)
TABLE 93-1 SIMPLE BEDSIDE EXAMINATION FINDINGS AND THEIR MEANING
*Complex regional pain syndrome
**Post-herpetic neuralgia
Stage 1: Nociception
A sufficiently strong noxious stimulus (mechanical, thermal, or chemical) activates nociceptors leading to depolarization of pain afferents (A delta and C fibers) that transmit the pain message from the peripheral tissue to the dorsal horn of the spinal cord. If the signal is strong enough (many nociceptive neurons firing) and/or repetitive enough, second-order neurons depolarize, sending the message rostrally to the lateral and medial thalamus. There, projections to the somatosensory cortex convey localization and intensity information resulting in the conscious perception of pain. Projections from the medial thalamus to the limbic system, specifically the anterior cingulate cortex and insular cortex, activate suffering and the emotional aspect of pain, as in Figure 93-1. Once this entire system is activated, repetitive nociceptor stimulation in the periphery can trigger a hyperresponse of the second-order neuron, increasing pain despite no increase in nociceptor input.
Stage 2: Peripheral Sensitization
The situation changes in Stage 2 pain when a noxious stimulus is very intense or prolonged leading to tissue damage and inflammation. Many nociceptors are inactive and unresponsive under normal circumstances; however, cell damage and death from injury or disease and accompanying inflammation can cause an “awakening” of nociceptors, which may spontaneously discharge and become more sensitive to peripheral stimulation (27,29). This is one type of peripheral sensitization. The sensitization of nociceptors occurs when such inflammatory mediators as bradykinin, prostaglandins, serotonin, and histamine (30) are released in damaged tissue. They bind to specific receptors on nociceptive afferents, which elicits the activation of second messenger systems. Ultimately, this leads to phosphorylation of receptors, influx of Ca2+ ions (which further sensitizes the cell), and release of such chemicals as substance P, which promote continued release of inflammatory mediators. Several types of pharmacologic receptors, including opioid, γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), bradykinin, histamine, serotonin, and, more recently, capsaicin (31), have also been identified on the surface membrane of sensory axons.
The consequence of peripheral sensitization is that depolarization of primary afferent pain fibers now occurs following stimuli that previously would have been subthreshold or even spontaneous, that is, sensitized nociceptors may fire without a noxious stimulus being present. The firing of an increased number of nociceptive afferents codes for an increase in the pain signal to the spinal cord and brain, causing increased pain from a given noxious stimulus—this is termed hyperalgesia.
Also in this circumstance, pain can result from normally innocuous light touch that activates sensitized peripheral neurons (32)—this is termed allodynia. A sunburn provides a good clinical example of a temporary state of peripheral sensitization, whereas arthritis would be a prolonged, chronic state as in Table 93-1.
Along with nociceptor sensitization, changes take place in the central pathways. The central pathways for processing nociceptive information begin at the level of the spinal cord (and medullary) dorsal horn. The interneuronal networks in the dorsal horn transmit nociceptive information not only to neurons that project to the brain but also to spinal motor neurons, which leads to enhanced reflex actions, including muscle spasm, in response to a noxious stimuli. Other inputs result in inhibition of projection neurons. The balance of these excitatory and inhibitory processes provides an explanatory basis for gate theory of pain transmission (33).
Following damage to peripheral nerves, whether by neurologic disease, amputations, surgery, radiation, or chemotherapy, pain fibers can fire spontaneously, can be triggered by trivial stimuli, and can fire in response to adrenergic outflow (such as occurs with psychological stress). Continual firing can lead to changes in the spinal cord including alterations of modulatory systems, central sensitization, and neuroplasticity, whereby dendritic formation in polymodal afferent fibers contributes to pain sensation, causing allodynia. These processes are illustrated by Figure 93-2 (34,35).
State 3: Central Sensitization
In central sensitization, persistent noxious input sets off a process of enhancement of responsiveness in the dorsal horn neurons that continues independent of primary afferent drive. Central sensitization is thought to be responsible for the clinical presentation of secondary hyperalgesia (hyperalgesia in sites away from the site of initial injury) and allodynia. The neurotransmitters contained in the terminals of nociceptive afferents in the dorsal horn include excitatory amino acids, particularly glutamate, as well as neuropeptides such as substance P, calcitonin gene–related peptide, vasoactive intestinal peptide, somatostatin, and others. The rationale for preemptive analgesia, when analgesics and/or neural blocks are given prior to a surgical procedure, is that such interventions are thought to prevent central sensitization (36). Inhibition of nociceptive circuits, mediated by a number of neurotransmitters, including amino acids such as 5-hydroxytryptamine, GABA, and glycine as well as neuropeptides such as enkephalins, forms the basis of many pharmacologic interventions.
Stage 3 pain states are abnormal pain states, chronic pain conditions that are often grouped as maldynia (bad pain). This term was proposed to better encompass not only the neuropathic pain (primary maldynia) initiated or caused by a primary lesion or dysfunction in the nervous systems but also the pain resulting results from inadequately relieved persistent nociceptive stimulation, also defined as secondary maldynia (37).
These chronic pain states are generally the consequence of chronic inflammation secondary to structure injury (e.g., arthritis), damage to peripheral nerves (e.g., neuropathy), damage to the CNS (e.g., thalamic stroke, multiple sclerosis), or changes in the CNS itself, for example, neuroplasticity, central sensitization, and alteration in modulatory systems like descending inhibitory systems (e.g., complex regional pain syndrome, as in Fig. 93-2). Maldynia is characterized by a lack of correlation between the intensity of a peripheral stimulus and the intensity of pain (manifest by exaggerated responses to noxious stimuli) and also by pain that is spontaneous or triggered by innocuous physical or psychological stimuli (38).
Descending control from supraspinal centers inhibits ascending nociceptive transmission, and this control is proportional to the afferent nociceptive input by C fibers (35).
It has been suggested that loss of a proportion of the normal input after peripheral nerve damage produces a lifting of descending inhibition, which could be responsible for enhanced pain perception, particularly for the phenomenon of hyperalgesia, in which there is an exaggerated pain response to a noxious stimulus (27).
However, the development of stage 3 pain may also involve genetic, cognitive, and emotional factors that remain to be clarified (33,39).
Recent brain imaging studies suggest that more rostral changes in the CNS also mediate sensitization. For example, changes in the hippocampus may sustain potentiation of an initial stimulus for up to a year, which can be reversed by placing laboratory mammals in a challenging novel environment (40). This finding supports a widely held clinical belief that stimulating, goal-directed activity and exercise suppress pain, enable improvements in functional ability, and even reduce the manifestations of central sensitization (e.g., allodynia and hyperpathia). This phenomenon is thought to be one reason for the effectiveness of comprehensive rehabilitation programs in treating chronic pain. Regions in the cingulate gyrus have now been identified that are responsible for modulating behavioral responses to pain, attention to pain, pain induced fear, and learning and pain (41–45). Figure 93-3 illustrates the sequence of biopsychosocial processes involved in the transition from acute injury to maldynia and its related comorbidities and disability. Treatment hence involves a comprehensive approach that identifies the patient’s biopsychosocial profile in this phenomenologic pathway and addresses specific pathophysiologic factors that perpetuate pain and pain-related and disease-related impairments and comorbidities (see Fig. 93-3).
Pain and Emotions
Emotional states that activate sympathetic arousal, such as anxiety or anger, can increase acute pain and reactivate or worsen chronic pain. When depression and anxiety disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and maldynias, are comorbid, they complicate each other’s treatment, such that treating one condition without treating the other effectively increases the likelihood of treatment failure. Moreover, certain personal traits, such as external locus of control (46,47) and a tendency to catastrophize (48–50), also predict worse outcomes. Finally, comorbid substance abuse may complicate the use of opioid analgesics for pain control. Substance abuse, by interfering with the sustained, goal-oriented behavior needed for functional restoration, leads to poor outcomes of pain treatment generally. The mechanisms for this effect will be discussed in more detail below. Thus, identifying and managing these comorbidities are critical to effectively treating pain.
Historical Notes on Psychosomatic Concepts
A review of rigorous epidemiologic studies over several decades demonstrates a strong association between depressive and anxiety disorders and chronic pain (50,51). Although chronic pain generally affects psychological health and functional status consistently across cultures (52), the relationship is more complex when considering specific painful diseases. For example, a mechanism for the high association between depression, anxiety disorders, and arthritis is suggested by recent studies linking the presence of depression to an impaired capacity to regulate the inflammatory cascade during stress (53).
The causal direction of the relationships among chronic pain disorders and psychiatric comorbidities and the relative influence of environmental factors and genetic/familial factors, “nature versus nurture,” are also being studied. For example, a series of studies of patients with myofascial facial pain demonstrated that major depression was more common than in community controls (54), that pain and depressed mood seasonally covaried (55), and, using a family study methodology, that the stress of living with chronic pain increased the risk for depression in persons without a personal or family history of depression (56). This latter study suggests that the stress of living with pain, not familial risk, is the dominant cause of comorbid depression in chronic facial pain. However, using a similarly rigorous methodology, a series of family studies of community-dwelling adult females with a different pain disease, fibromyalgia, a disorder marked by generalized muscle pain and tenderness with no peripheral pathology, supports the hypothesis that fibromyalgia is a depressive spectrum disorder (57). This and other studies (58) suggest a shared pathogenesis, such as dysfunction in serotonin and norepinephrine systems, in fibromyalgia (59–62), and in depression (63,64). Shared genetic risk factors for fibromyalgia and major depressive disorder may also be identified, for example, genetic polymorphisms in serotonin-related genes in fibromyalgia (65–68). Thus, the increase in rates of major depression in families of persons with fibromyalgia, with or without depression, may reflect vulnerability in these neurotransmitter systems that influence both disorders.
The effects of environmental factors on the pathogenesis of pain disorders and depressive disorders may be shared. Stress and trauma, implicated in both facial pain (56) and depression (69–72), may alter the course of pain in subjects with existing pain. In a prospective study of a community sample of fibromyalgia and major life trauma (the 9/11 World Trade Center attack), exposure to stress was not associated with an increase in rates of fibromyalgia in controls (community subjects without fibromyalgia) but was associated with increases in pain in community subjects with fibromyalgia already confirmed by research diagnostic criteria (73).
One interpretation of these aforementioned studies is that central states such as fibromyalgia and major depressive disorder (MDD) share a genetic or other biologically mediated vulnerability to respond to stressful or traumatic events with psychological and pain-related symptoms. This interpretation is consistent with two recent studies, one showing that persons with fibromyalgia symptoms are more likely than community controls to develop PTSD in response to a fixed level of exposure to traumatic events
(73) and another demonstrating that during experimental interpersonal stress, women with fibromyalgia who were primed to experience negative mood prior to stress showed greater subsequent pain elevations than similarly primed women with osteoarthritis (74). Genetic studies suggest that a functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene affects the influence of stressful life events on depression (75), a region that has also been implicated in some investigations of fibromyalgia (66,67).
Pain and Anxiety
The experimental and epidemiologic evidence points to a strong association between anxiety and pain and anxiety disorders and pain (50,51) mediated in part by the amygdala, which appears to play a key role in the regulation of the emotional response to pain. The “nociceptive amygdala” can be influenced by a wide range of environmental and internal stimuli to modulate the subjective experience of pain. The hippocampus, a structure with robust connections to the amygdala, is a center for memory formation, storage, and retrieval. It provides information from detailed memories that are processed by the amygdala and given a particular emotional value, which may influence the development of “pain memory” after central sensitization (40). The emotional value the amygdala places on a particular memory is then fed back to the hippocampus, which integrates this information and either strengthens or weakens the memory. This is why it is believed that events associated with high emotional content, and often painful injury, such as a car accident or being wounded in battle, tend to be remembered in greater detail, as observed in PTSD, than those experiences with little emotional significance (back pain on your drive to work every morning or working in the garden on the weekend). Exposure to novel, “interesting” environments can reverse hippocampal sensitization in pain experiments (40). Studies of disorders such as complex regional pain syndrome (formerly called reflex sympathetic dystrophy or RSD) show that specific sequences of mental activities (16,76) and biofeedback using brain imaging (17) reduce pain. These studies support the usefulness of rehabilitative approaches emphasizing patient involvement in goal-oriented, motivating, engrossing activities (18,19).
PTSD, which is often comorbid with chronic pain associated with physical injury, is associated with changes in the amygdala, hippocampus, and other areas of the limbic system (the neural circuits of complex emotional experience). Neuroimaging studies have found consistent reductions in either total hippocampal volume or blood flow in men and women with PTSD (77–80). influence the amygdala. Human imaging studies have found that the fusiform gyrus, prefrontal gyrus, and anterior cingulate gyrus are preferentially activated in response to fearful stimuli (81,82). The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), which is involved in the evaluation of risk and reward and social norms, may also have a direct role in regulation of anxiety via its connection to the amygdala (83). The cortex, then, plays an essential role in the categorization, appraisal, and attenuation of our reactions to fearful stimuli, such as pain. These connections are outlined in Figure 93-4. The higher cortical connections to the more primitive fight, flight, and reward circuitry provide a degree of conscious recognition and control over these processes. These connections and their conditioning suggest the biologic basis for the effects of behavioral treatments used widely in chronic pain, such as relaxation and biofeedback. They also suggest specific targets for neuromodulation.
FIGURE 93-4 The circuitry of fear. (From Tirado CF, Gallagher RM. The diagnosis of anxiety and anxiety disorders in chronic spinal pain. In: Slipman C, Wetzel T eds. Interventional spine: an algorithmic approach. New York: Elsevier Health Sciences, 2007.)
More recent functional brain chemistry research has provided neuroanatomical evidence for the overlap between the processing and perception of pain and anxiety. In an experiment comparing patients with chronic low back pain (CLBP) to normal controls, significant differences were found in two regions of the association cortex (orbitofrontal [OFC] and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [DLPFC]), the cingulate gyrus (part of the limbic system), and the thalamus (84). The study showed that persons with CLBP have differences in regional brain chemistry in the OFC and DLPFC when compared with those without pain. Additionally, persons with CLBP and anxiety had changes in chemistry suggesting increased interaction among all four brain regions, whereas anxious controls only had changes observed in the OFC. Anxiety and pain, therefore, share common neurochemical pathways and can interact in a way that leads to the reorganization of perceptual pathways in the brain. Living with chronic pain is stressful and is thought to be associated with physiologic and psychological changes, yet there is a knowledge gap regarding brain elements involved in such conditions. Baliki and Apkarian’s studies with fMRI suggested that the subjective spontaneous pain of chronic back pain involves specific spatiotemporal neuronal mechanisms, distinct from those observed for acute experimental pain, implicating a salient role for the emotional brain concerning the self (85).
CLINICAL INTERFACE BETWEEN PAIN AND ADDICTION
Multiple points of interface exist between pain and addiction, and these become significant at the clinical level. Pain and drug reward share common neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates, and the physiologic sequela of addiction (i.e., tolerance, dependence, altered stress response) have clear effects on pain management. Drugs of abuse often have analgesic properties, yet the disease of addiction brings with it malaise, mood states, behaviors, and social losses that serve to worsen the pain experience. The clinical interaction of pain and addiction is particularly complex in the case of addiction to opioids, as these drugs appear to be imbued with both analgesic and hyperalgesic properties. Further, accumulating genetic data suggest that pain, opioid analgesia, and opioid addiction may share similar patterns of gene expression, which become evident in the response of the individual patient.
Neurobiologic Mechanisms of Addiction
Like pain, addiction is an extremely complex human response, with strong behavioral components, that cannot be entirely understood yet by analyzing its physiology. Addiction can be defined as a chronic, relapsing disorder that has been characterized by (a) a compulsion to seek and take drugs, (b) loss of control over drug intake, and (c) emergence of a negative emotional state (e.g., dysphoria, anxiety, and irritability) that defines a motivational withdrawal syndrome when access to the drug is prevented (86). The occasional, limited, recreational use of a drug is clinically distinct from the loss of control over drug intake and the emergence of compulsive drug-seeking behavior that characterize addiction.
Addiction has been conceptualized as a three-stage cycle—binge/intoxication, withdrawal/negative affect, and preoccupation/anticipation—that worsens over time and involves dramatic changes in the brain reward and stress systems (87). Two primary sources of reinforcement, positive and negative reinforcement, have been hypothesized to play a role in the development of addiction. Positive reinforcement is defined as the process by which presentation of a stimulus increases the probability of a response; negative reinforcement is defined as the process by which removal of an aversive stimulus (or aversive state in the case of addiction) increases the probability of a response. Different theoretical perspectives from experimental psychology (positive and negative reinforcement frameworks), social psychology (self-regulation failure framework), and neurobiology (counteradaptation and sensitization frameworks) can be superimposed on the stages of the addiction cycle (86). These stages are thought to feed into each other, become more intense, and ultimately lead to the pathologic state known as addiction. Standard diagnostic criteria for addictive disorders (DSM-IV-TR) (88) not only rely on the more overt physiologic responses to chronic drug use (two of seven criteria such as tolerance and withdrawal) but also focus on such behavioral consequences as loss of control over drug use and significant disruptions in social and occupational functioning (five of seven criteria). Ultimately, understanding of the molecular, cellular, and neurocircuitry mechanisms underlying addictive disorders will provide a heuristic framework for better diagnoses.
A discussion of the physiologic mechanisms underlying addictive disease is provided in Chapters 1, 3, and 4 of this textbook as well as others. Discussion is provided therein regarding the different sites and unique mechanisms of action that drugs of abuse have in the central nervous system, underlying their acute effects and distinctive withdrawal syndromes. However, there are some common elements to addiction that are particularly relevant to the intersection of pain and addiction.
The overall hypothesis argued here is that drug addiction represents a break from homeostatic brain regulatory mechanisms that control the normal everyday emotional states of the animal, and this break in homeostasis is known as allostasis. Allostasis can be defined simply as “the ability to achieve stability through change” (88). A more complete definition is “the concept of physiology where an organism must vary all of the parameters of its internal milieu and match them appropriately to perceived and anticipated environmental demands in order to maintain stability” (88). More specifically, two components are hypothesized to comprise the allostatic break from homeostasis in addiction: underactivation of brain reward transmitter function and recruitment of brain stress (or antireward) systems. These systems (reward and stress) interact at all stages of the addiction cycle to potentiate such emotional dysregulation. Additionally, drug addiction, similar to other chronic physiologic disorders such as high blood pressure, worsens over time, is subject to significant environmental influences, and leaves a residual neuroadaptive trace that allows rapid “read-diction” even after detoxification and years of abstinence. These characteristics of drug addiction imply more than simply a homeostatic dysregulation of hedonic function but rather a dynamic break with homeostasis of these systems toward a new set point, termed allostasis as noted above.
Of relevance to issues of pain and pain treatment, the neurobiology of addiction often is characterized by two incompletely understood yet related allostatic states: tolerance and dependence. This new level of functioning that is present in addiction may be manifest not only in those brain systems in which drugs of abuse exert discrete actions (within-system neuroadaptations) but also in brain systems that oppose the actions of the drug (between-system neuroadaptations), as elaborated below (89). Importantly, the simple presence of these adaptations in the nervous system does not infer addiction. A pain patient may be tolerant to or dependent on the effects of an opioid without being addicted to it. Addiction is identified by a cluster of aberrant patterns of behavior that, while partially motivated by these physiologic changes, is evident in much broader domains; see above.
Tolerance
Ongoing use of all psychoactive drugs with addiction potential results in the development of drug tolerance, which is defined as a reduction in response to a given dose of drug after repeated administration (90,91). The neuroadaptations associated with tolerance counter the acute drug effects to maintain system-level homeostasis. A theoretical explanation for the processes underlying tolerance is offered by the opponent process theory of acquired motivation (92). Reflecting homeostatic assumptions, the theory describes how, over the course of repeated exposures to an affectively charged stimulus, a counteracting or opposing emotional response develops, which eventually accounts for habituation to the stimulus and becomes the predominant feeling state in its absence. Although opponent process theory was initially rooted in a behavioral construct, Koob et al. (93) advanced the theory by providing evidence for a neurobiologic basis to the opponent processes of tolerance and dependence in the case of addiction and specifically, for our purposes, in the case of opioid dependence. Their model predicts that in order to maintain a “normal” or homeostatic level of reward system activity, “antireward” systems are recruited to counteract drug effects, which become stronger with each exposure and extinguish more slowly than the original response (94,95). Upon abrupt drug withdrawal, the tolerance-producing processes are revealed.
Tolerance involves adaptations that occur both at the site of drug action (receptor or ion channel) (termed within system) and in systems distal to the site of drug action (96,97) (termed between system) (89). For example, tolerance to opioids is evident both at the level of opioid receptor transduction in the nucleus accumbens reward pathways and in the postsynaptic neuron at the site of this discrete drug action. Because drugs typically act at selective receptors, tolerance has been conceptualized as a functional “uncoupling” of the receptor from its effector response (opening or closing an ion channel, initiating second messenger systems); in other words, a certain proportion of receptors are rendered less functional or nonfunctional, thus making the drug less effective (97,98). An alternative form of tolerance is a between-system adaptation in which another neurotransmitter circuit becomes activated in response to sedative action in an attempt to restore homeostasis (89). Activation of an arousal system such as glutamate in the face of a sedative hypnotic drug that becomes particularly manifest during withdrawal would be an obvious example. Clinically, the resulting tolerance provides a certain amount of protection for the user, such as with the respiratory depressant effects of opioids or the anesthetic effects of ethanol.
Dependence
A related consequence of chronic drug use is dependence, which is an altered physiologic state in which an effect opposite to that of a drug becomes manifest when the drug is removed. This, of course, is intimately linked to tolerance. When a drug blood level falls below a critical point, the adaptive changes associated with tolerance predominate and become profoundly nonadaptive (99). Suddenly unopposed by drug effects, the sources of tolerance become evident as the characteristic drug-specific withdrawal syndrome (100,101) and a more generalized negative emotional state. Such negative emotional states include malaise, anxiety, dysphoria, emotional pain, loss of interest in natural rewards, and depression; are common to withdrawal to all drugs of abuse; and are particularly relevant to the interface of addiction and chronic pain.
Neurobiologic Interface of Pain and Addiction
The development of aversive or negative emotional states that constitute negative reinforcers in addiction has been linked to two distinct neurobiologic processes: loss of function in the reward systems (within-system neuroadaptations) and recruitment of brain stress or antireward systems (between-system neuroadaptations) (86,89). As addiction develops, brain stress systems that elicit release of such chemicals as corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), norepinephrine, and dynorphin are recruited, producing aversive or stress-like states (102,103). At the same time, within the positive motivational circuits of the ventral striatum–extended amygdala, reward function is diminished. The combination of decreases in reward neurotransmitter function and potentiation of brain stress systems provides a powerful source of negative reinforcement that contributes to compulsive drug-seeking behavior and long-term dependence. Obviously, the pain itself generates a negative emotional state that can exacerbate the negative emotional state of addiction and vice versa.
Analgesic Effects of Drugs of Abuse
As points of interface between the physiology of pain and addiction are considered, it is important to recognize that many classes of abused drugs have analgesic properties. The opioids are defined by their direct analgesic effects, and, at high doses, alcohol is a potent anesthetic and analgesic. The sedative hypnotics, particularly benzodiazepines, are used to treat the pain sustained by muscle spasticity secondary to upper motor neuron damage (104,105) and are a standard anxiolytic adjunct for procedural sedation and analgesia (106,107). CNS stimulants, such as cocaine and caffeine, produce and potentiate analgesia, presumably by increasing neurotransmitter activity in descending inhibitory pain pathways.
Of current interest are the effects of cannabinoids on pain perception. In 1992, Devane et al. (108) developed a high-affinity synthetic cannabinoid ligand, confirming the presence of endogenous cannabinoid G protein–coupled receptors in the human body (108), with central subtype receptors widely distributed in the cortex, basal ganglia, cerebellum, and hippocampus (109,110). More recently, Mao et al. (111) provided good preclinical evidence for an independent THC-responsive antinociceptive pathway that is particularly effective for pain of neuropathic origin. While clinical trials of cannabinoids in multiple sclerosis have suggested a benefit for neuropathic pain (112,113), human studies of cannabinoid-mediated analgesia have been limited by study size, heterogeneous patient populations, subjective outcome measures, and variable drug pharmacokinetics (114). Interference with glutamate release at the level of the dorsal root ganglia or periaqueductal gray is one hypothesized mechanism by which cannabinoids provide analgesia (115,116).
General Effects of Addiction on Pain
Careful examination of the extant literature provides evidence that physiologic states consistent with drug addiction can affect nociceptive input, processing, and/or modulation in several different ways. Addictive disease facilitates the experience of pain in multiple ways, including its associated sympathetic arousal and negative mood states. These changes are related to both the discrete effects of certain classes of drugs and the effects of all drugs of abuse on reward-relevant systems.
For example, important psychological sequelae, including sleep disorders and psychiatric illness, are characteristic of addiction and, as previously noted, can contribute to the experience of chronic pain and decrease the efficacy of analgesic interventions. Addiction commonly co-occurs with anxiety and affective disorders (117,118), which—if not corrected—can increase the perception of pain. Depression has been demonstrated to increase the discomfort associated with pain and to impair function in studies of chronic pain patients. Symptoms frequently improve with effective antidepressant treatment (119–121).
In addiction, drug use is characterized by frequent and rapid fluctuations in blood levels of the drug. Abused substances tend to be ingested in short-acting formulations and via routes with rapid onset (i.e., inhalation, intravenous) to boost the psychoactive effect. These use patterns result in rapidly alternating states of intoxication and subtle (or sometimes full-blown) withdrawal (see above).
Further, a strong and persistent negative affective state accompanies withdrawal from many drugs of abuse. When drug-free, the addicted individual suffers from negative symptoms such as anhedonia, prolonged dysphoria, and irritability (122,123), which have been attributed to dopamine (DA) depletion in the reward pathways and also to recruitment of the brain stress/antireward systems. A second component of the negative emotional states that is common to all drugs of abuse is the activation of brain stress systems such as corticotropin-releasing factor, norepinephrine, and dynorphin. Clearly, the negative feeling states associated with drug withdrawal can augment the subjective discomfort associated with pain.
Finally, and not insignificantly, the interpersonal conflicts, role adjustments, and social support losses that characterize the social context of addiction can worsen the experience of pain, making the individual less able to manage or cope with discomfort. Further, the chaotic and drug-oriented lifestyle of the addict makes it difficult to comply with prescribed pain management regimes. Empirical support for a worsened pain experience in individuals with addiction has been demonstrated in the case of experimental pain, as compared to normal controls. Persons currently using addictive drugs are significantly less tolerant of cold pressor (CP) pain, regardless of the particular drug of abuse (124–126).
Potential Neurobiologic Basis for Overlap of the Allostatic States of Addiction and Pain
The neural substrates underlying allostatic emotional changes seen in addiction include decreases in reward function mediated by neurochemical changes in the ventral striatum (molecular neuroadaptations in medium spiny neurons and loss of function of the DA system) and increases in brain stress system function meditated by neurochemical changes in the extended amygdala (recruitment of corticotropin-releasing factor, dynorphin, and norepinephrine) (94,127). Links have been hypothesized to exist between the neural mechanisms responsible for a hypersensitive negative emotional state (termed “hyperkatifeia”) and opioid-induced hyperalgesia (OIH) (128) or maldynia (see above). For example, evidence suggests that the neural substrates of stress system neuroadaptations associated with addiction may overlap with substrates of emotional aspects of pain processing in areas such as the amygdala (127). The spino-(trigemino)-pontoamygdaloid pathway, which projects from the dorsal horn of the spinal cord to the mesencephalic parabrachial area and then to the central nucleus of the amygdala, has been implicated in processing emotional components of pain (129,130). Pain-responsive neurons are also abundant in the lateral part of the central nucleus of the amygdala (131), an area that also may be responsible for negative emotional responses to abused drugs (132,133). Perhaps even more compelling, opioid withdrawal and alcohol withdrawal in animal models of compulsive-like self-administration produce increased anxiety-like responses and hyperalgesia, both of which are blocked by CRF antagonists (134).
One hypothesis to explain the cross talk between opioid addiction and chronic pain syndromes is that some patients may be more prone to the development of hyperkatifeia during withdrawal. An allostatic view would suggest that opioid-induced hyperkatifeia would be much less likely to occur when the opioid is restoring homeostasis by relieving pain. In this framework, the presence of pain would minimize activation of the opponent process (hyperalgesia, maldynia, and, concomitantly, hyperkatifeia), and long-term pain control could be achieved with stable doses of analgesics. Thus, one hypothesis is that a hypernegative emotional state or hyperkatifeia to opioids is most likely to occur when the opioid produces a break with homeostasis (94). The hypothesis would be that hyperalgesia–maldynia/ hyperkatifeia is much less likely to occur when the opioid is restoring homeostasis, such as during pain treatment. However, if excessive opioids are administered, because of overdosing, rapid escalation (overshooting), pharmaco-kinetic variables, or genetic sensitivity, then the body will react to that perturbation with the engagement of the opponent processes of hyperalgesia–maldynia and hyperkatifeia mediated by significant cross talk in a brain structures such as the central nucleus of the amygdala. Repeated engagement of opponent processes without time for the system to reestablish homeostasis will engage the allostatic mechanisms described above. Such a framework suggests that manifestation of OIH has important clinical implications: (a) The opioid has exceeded the amount that is effective for pain control, and (b) susceptible individuals are at risk for developing hyperkatifeia, the unstable emotional and behavioral state underlying addiction (128).
Unique Effects of Opioid Addiction on Pain
The effects of addictive disease on pain become especially pertinent in the case of individuals addicted to opioid drugs because the class of drug abused also is the primary pharmacologic tool for the treatment of moderate to severe clinical pain. Agonist activity at mu opioid receptors provides a robust and reliable analgesia, making opioids the most powerful and effective treatment for pain known to man.
Opioid addiction and opioid analgesia are dependent upon opioid agonist activity at the mu opioid receptor; the reinforcing and analgesic effects of morphine, for example, can be blocked by the administration of mu receptor antagonists and are absent in mu opioid receptor gene knockout mice. Mu opioid receptors are found throughout body tissues, however, and effects of opioid administration can include activity on immune, respiratory, and gut functions (135). In that the same receptor is central to both addiction and pain systems, it is reasonable to expect that perturbations in the latter might be evident in individuals chronically exposed to opioids (agonists or antagonists) in the context of the former. Although drug reward and analgesia are distinct processes, opioids activate their shared anatomical substrate, the mu opioid receptor, inducing the previously described interrelated CNS changes of tolerance and dependence. Individuals with opioid addiction may seek psychoactive effects, yet are not immune to the effects of these drugs on central and peripheral opioid-relevant pain systems.
That opioid addiction and pain responses might be interrelated is not a new idea. In his early essay describing his clinical observations on patients injecting morphine on a daily basis, physician and author Clifford Albutt (1870) asked, “Does morphia (sic) tend to encourage the very pain it pretends to relieve?” He continues, “I have much reason to suspect that a reliance upon hypodermic morphia only ended in a curious state of perpetuated pain” (136). Although poorly explored in the subsequent clinical research, questions about the pain responses of opioid-dependent patients again arose with the institution of methadone maintenance in the 1960s.
Pain and Methadone Maintenance Patients
As early as 1965, Martin and Inglis (136) described significantly lower tolerance for cold pressor–induced pain in a sample of incarcerated, known opioid-abusing females, in comparison to matched nonaddicted controls. Ho and Dole (137) found that both methadone-maintained (MM) and drug-free opioid-addicted individuals had significantly lower thresholds for CP pain than did matched nonaddicted sibling controls. Subsequent work supports that, at methadone trough conditions, CP pain threshold does not differ between MM and drug-free opioid addicts but is significantly lower for MM patients in comparison to matched normal controls (124,125,138). Under the same conditions, MM patients’ CP pain tolerance is less than that in both matched drug-free patients with addiction disorder (121) and matched controls (124,138). A similar nonsignificant trend for decreased pain tolerance in MM patients was noted for electrical pain stimulation, although a significant analgesic effect for methadone on CP, electrical, and pressure pain 2 to 4 hours postdose was reported (124,138). With respect to perceived pain severity, Schall et al. (139) found no difference between MM and control subjects in their perception of pressure pain (measured on a scale of 1 to 10) immediately prior to methadone dosing.
These cross-sectional data indicate that MM patients are more sensitive to pain than are matched normal controls. Across electrical and thermal (cold) nociceptive stimuli, methadone patients reliably demonstrate poor tolerance for experimental pain and are, on average, between 42% to 76%, less tolerant of CP pain than are normal controls matched on age, gender, and ethnicity (124,125,138,140,141).With appreciable (albeit trough) methadone blood levels, these patients not only receive no analgesic effect from daily, high-dose methadone but present a case for the antianalgesic (hyperalgesic) effects of chronic methadone therapy. Pilot data suggest that degree of hyperalgesia varies with the intrinsic activity of the opioid maintenance agent; patients maintained on the partial agonist buprenorphine for the treatment of opioid addiction are less hyperalgesic than those maintained on methadone, a full agonist (140).
Opioid-Induced Hyperalgesia (OIH)
Accumulated evidence indicates that opioid administration not only provides analgesia but concurrently sets into motion certain antianalgesic or hyperalgesic processes. Ongoing work has culminated the current understanding of what is described as OIH or diminished tolerance for pain following acute and chronic opioid administration (142–149). From this perspective, the pain intolerance of MM patients reflects a latent hyperalgesia secondary to chronic opioid exposure. Across this literature, it is of note that the degree of hyperalgesic response to opioid administration (~30% of baseline) is quite reliable.
Withdrawal Hyperalgesia
The observation that chronic opioid administration induces increased pain sensitivity or hyperalgesia is not a new one. The phenomenon of hyperalgesia has long been recognized as a fundamental symptom of the opioid withdrawal syndrome in animal models of dependence (150–154), and although not extensively studied, hyperalgesia and spontaneous muscle and bone pain have long been considered cardinal symptoms of opioid withdrawal in humans (90,155). The time course, opioid dose–response relationship, and opioid pretreatment parameters of withdrawal hyperalgesia have been carefully characterized in preclinical models for over 30 years, such that it arises following single or chronic opioid exposure, can be detected up to 5 days following subcutaneous injection, and increases in intensity with pretreatment opioid dose or intrinsic efficacy (155–157).
Further characterization of the hyperalgesia following opioid administration confirmed that although it is present during withdrawal, OIH exists independent of the withdrawal syndrome and can, in fact, be detected in the presence of opioid analgesia (158–160). Demonstrating the key role of the mu opioid receptor in the development of this hyperalgesia, investigators found diminished OIH development in mouse strains with reduced mu opioid receptor binding. More recent work has shown that OIH increases in severity with intermittent or naloxone-interrupted opioid dosing (147,161–163). A biphasic response to opioid administration is described in which analgesia is an early response, followed by the longer-lasting hyperalgesic state (161,164).
Heightened pain perception has been long observed in addicted persons during abstinence (165). Patients on methadone maintenance have low pain tolerance (166), and pain is one of the main triggers of relapse to addiction in methadone-maintained individuals. Also, former opioid-addicted individuals maintained on either methadone or the partial agonist buprenorphine showed increased sensitivity to cold pressor pain (125). Indeed, Ren et al. (167) found that a hyperalgesic state can persist for up to 5 months in abstinent opiate addicts and that addicts with more pain sensitivity also displayed greater cue-induced craving at this time point. Thus, opioid-addicted individuals with poor pain tolerance may suffer a more severe form of addiction, have difficulty tolerating the discomfort (pain) inherent in detoxification and early abstinence, and be more likely to relapse.
OIH and Tolerance
The presence of hyperalgesia with ongoing opioid use demands reconsideration of the well-described phenomenon of analgesic tolerance. Jasinski (168) suggests that “tolerance occurs not at the (opioid) receptor level but occurs from increased activity of other functional systems to counteract the effects of opioids” (p. 185). The hyperalgesic processes initiated by opioid administration serves to counteract opioid analgesia. As eloquently hypothesized by Colpaert (169,170) and Celerier (161,171–173), that which appears to be opioid analgesic tolerance, and therefore increased opioid need, may in fact be an organismic response to an opioid-induced hypersensitivity to pain or “apparent tolerance.” Essentially, opioids lose their analgesic effectiveness in the face of decreased tolerance for pain. That OIH might contribute to the variable and incompletely understood phenomenon of analgesic tolerance in the clinical setting is a paradigm-shifting idea. Analgesic tolerance has long been an untested concern for withholding opioids in the treatment of chronic pain (174,175); reconceptualizing tolerance as a reflection of OIH in the study of chronic pain management may lead to novel insights into the utility of chronic opioid therapy for this patient population. Thus, analgesic tolerance may reflect underlying neuroadaptive changes that reflect the possibility of OIH.
Through their preclinical work exploring the molecular mechanisms of hyperalgesia, Mao et al. (176–180) demonstrated the similarities between opioid analgesic tolerance and OIH. In an important series of studies, these investigators provide credible evidence that the development of opioid analgesic tolerance via intermittent morphine dosing induces hyperalgesia, while animals made hyperalgesic via neuropathic injury concomitantly exhibit opioid analgesic tolerance (results recently were replicated with heroin by Celerier, Laulin et al. (172)).
Mechanisms of OIH
Various physiologic explanations for the development of OIH have been offered and have focused on both spinal and supraspinal systems as the site of action (151,156,181–187). In the work described above, Mao and colleagues demonstrated that the common pathway for the development of morphine tolerance/hyperalgesia is activation of ionotropic NMDA receptors on dorsal horn spinal cord neurons, with subsequent intracellular increases in protein kinase C and nitric oxide. OIH is conceptualized as a variant of central sensitization and, like the hyperalgesia of neuropathic origin (177,188), can be prevented by N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonism and calcium channel blockers (162,171,189–192). The latter finding has spurred interest in the potential utility of NMDA receptor antagonists as a means to enhance the effectiveness of opioid analgesia (91,193–196) and complements the ongoing work of addiction scientists on the utility of these agents to reverse opioid tolerance and dependence (197–201).
Alternatively, Porreca et al. have provided good preclinical evidence (187) (3E) that OIH may in fact be the result of the activation of descending pain facilitation systems arising from mu opiate receptor activation (162) in the rostral ventromedial medulla (RVM) (160,183,187,202). Specifically implicated are opioid-induced increased levels of the pronociceptive peptide cholecystokinin (CCK) in the RVM. Increases in CCK appear to play a role in the development of opiate analgesic tolerance as well (203). It is suggested that CCK activity in the medulla drives descending pain facilitatory mechanisms, resulting in spinal hyperalgesic responses to nociceptive input (186,187).
Various spinal neuropeptides, distinct from excitatory amino acid systems, have also been implicated in the development of OIH. Over a decade ago, Simonnet’s laboratory showed that a single dose of parenteral heroin resulted in significant release of the antiopioid neuropeptide FF from the spinal cord in rats, an effect blocked by the subsequent administration of the opioid antagonist naloxone. This induced a hyperalgesia 30% below threshold baseline (204). More recent animal work in Porreca’s laboratory has demonstrated increased levels of lumbar dynorphin, a kappa opioid agonist with pronociceptive activity, following sustained spinal opioid administration (182,186). Interestingly, the hyperalgesic effects of opioids have been reversed by the administration of an antagonist to the neurokinin-1 receptor, which is the receptor mediating the nociceptive neuropeptide substance P (183). Particularly active in pain of inflammatory origin, substance P involvement suggests a neuroinflammatory component to the development of OIH (160).
Emerging evidence suggests a key role for neuroimmune processes in the development OIH (205,206). In this model, exogenously administered opioids are theorized to bind to μ opioid receptors located on the astrocytes of the blood–brain barrier, activating these and resulting in the subsequent expression and release of proinflammatory chemokines and cytokines. Specifically, peripheral immune cells activated in response to opioid administration are hypothesized to bind to glial cells and induce specific classes of central proinflammatory (and therefore pronociceptive) cytokines, thus resulting in a state of heightened pain sensitivity (205,207).
In support of this hypothesis, Song and Zhao (208) and Johnston and Westbrook (209) have demonstrated that the administration of a glial cell inhibitor (fluorocitrate) reversed the hyperalgesic effect of morphine in acute and chronically treated rats. Further, administration of a cytokine inhibitors interleukin-1β receptor antagonist and interleukin-6 neutralizing antibody has been shown to reverse and/or block morphine-induced hyperalgesia (185,210). As theorized by Watkins and Maier, peripheral inflammation via glial cell activation results in sensitization of spinal pain-processing pathways, both increasing the perception of pain and supporting functional “sickness behaviors” (205,207,211,212). Reviewing the literature, Ossipov et al. (161) commented that, “opioid-induced abnormal pain may share a molecular signature with pain of inflammatory origin” (p. 320).
Finally, more than 30 years ago, Seigel et al. demonstrated a conditioned component to OIH. A robust hyperalgesia was observed in animals receiving saline in an environment previously paired with morphine administration (213,214). This work showed that rats receiving acute morphine doses (3 to 9 doses separated by 48 hours) in a specific environment demonstrate significant hyperalgesia in the same setting as compared to rats receiving morphine unpaired with setting or saline control rats. Because conditioned responses to medications typically are opposite in direction to unconditioned drug effects, the learned responses were ascribed a causal role in the development of drug tolerance.
Clinical Evidence of OIH
As noted, evidence for the existence and characteristics of OIH has been principally established in animal models, making it difficult to extrapolate to the clinical model of the patient with chronic pain. Not only is pain a much more highly modulated experience in humans than in animals, but it is not entirely clear how pain tolerance in humans (point of subjective intolerance of pain, an indicator of hyperalgesia) maps onto putative pain threshold or perception (point at which animal withdraws tail, jumps, on hotplate) in animals. Further, the development of OIH has been better characterized in animals without pain or with acute pain; thus, its effect and relevance in the setting of clinical chronic pain remain incompletely understood.
Probably the best described is evidence for OIH in humans found in postoperative patients who received opioids intraoperatively and perhaps in a dose-dependent manner (215–218). These investigators showed that in patients undergoing various abdominal surgeries, postoperative reports of pain severity and/or opioid consumption were significantly higher in those patients receiving intrathecal or intravenous short-acting opioids (fentanyl and remi-fentanil) during surgery in comparison to those receiving placebo (216,217) or low-dose opioids (215,218). It is theorized that opioid exposure during surgery induced hyperal-gesic changes resulting in increased pain and opioid need in the postoperative period.
Reports of OIH in patients with chronic pain are much less common and are primarily limited to individuals with malignant pain. Across a number of case studies, the emergence of hyperalgesia and allodynia has been reported in cancer patients on large or rapidly escalating doses of morphine or fentanyl (219–222), in at least one case resolving with discontinuation and switching to a weaker opioid (223). A similar pattern of OIH induced with intrathecal sufentanil and clearing with opioid discontinuation has been described in a single case report of a woman with chronic nonmalignant low back pain (failed back syndrome) (224), suggesting that regardless of the etiology of chronic pain (malignant vs. nonmalignant), OIH becomes evident in certain individuals in the context of high-dose opioid therapy.
Perhaps of most relevance to the clinical situation of chronic pain and ongoing opioid therapy, a single study by Chu et al. (159) demonstrated the development of OIH in a small sample of patients with chronic nonmalignant low back pain following 1 month of oral sustained-release morphine treatment (median dose 75 mg/d). In these six individuals, 30 days of morphine at therapeutic doses not only resulted in analgesic tolerance to challenge doses of remifentanil but also diminished tolerance to CP pain by almost 25% from baseline. Despite appreciating analgesia, the experimental pain responses of these patients suggest the presence of hyperalgesic changes.
Indirect evidence of OIH may be reflected in the large number of chronic pain patients who do not respond to and/or ultimately discontinue chronic opioid treatment; the degree to which the development of OIH contributes to poor outcomes in these patients is unclear. As noted in several recent meta-analyses of the extant clinical trial data (225–228), the benefits of opioid therapy for chronic nonmalignant pain beyond 6 to 8 weeks of treatment have yet to be demonstrated; thus, potential long-term consequences of opioid therapy, such as OIH, have not been fully evaluated. High rates of dropout (~30%) are reported in these short-term trials (227,229,230), with rates of up to 56% in longer-term open-label follow-up studies (7 to 24 months) (227). Most commonly attributed to side effects (nausea, constipation, drowsiness, and dizziness), patients also report dropping out due to a perceived lack of efficacy, perhaps reflecting OIH. Interestingly, and in agreement with Chu et al. (159), two groups of investigators (231–233) report findings of pain increasing following opioid titration. Commenting on the large cross-sectional study of Eriksen et al. (226), which showed that patients on opioid therapy for the treatment of chronic nonmalignant pain were less likely to achieve key outcomes (pain relief, improved quality of life, and functionality) than those not on opioid therapy, Ballantyne (234) warns that “opioids are not a panacea for chronic pain” (p. 4).
Genetic Factors Underlying Pain and Addiction
Individual differences in pain tolerance and opioid response have long been appreciated at the clinical level, and the genetic factors, which underlie these differences, are increasingly elucidated. For example, heritable differences in hepatic P450 isoenzyme activity affect both the amount of reward and analgesia received from an opioid. Individuals who are extensive “metabolizers” of opioids (i.e., those with high P450 activity) receive less analgesia and reward from a given opioid dose (235–240) theoretically putting them at decreased risk for addiction but increased risk for unrelieved pain (241). Preliminary data suggest that these extensive metabolizers of opioids are less tolerant of CP pain, possibly due to defects in the endogenous synthesis of opioids (242,243).
Genetics of Pain and Addiction
An analysis of the preclinical literature, most notably from the laboratories of Mogil (244,245) and Elmer (246–248), reveals that recombinant murine strains differ both in their baseline tolerance for pain and in the amount of reward or reinforcement they enjoy from opioids. Those strains of animals with poor pain tolerance (i.e., C57, CXBK) find opioids to be highly reinforcing, whereas those with good pain tolerance (i.e., BALB/c, CXBH) receive little reinforcement from opioids (246,249,250). Further, pain-tolerant murine strains receive robust opioid analgesia and demonstrate increased opioid receptor binding activity as compared to pain-intolerant strains (251,252).
With the advent of molecular genetics, investigators in both the pain and substance abuse fields have been focusing on polymorphisms in the mu opioid gene receptor (OPRM) as candidates underlying phenotypes for pain sensitivity (241,253–255), opioid analgesic response (237–256), and addiction (257,258). Probably best characterized is the single nucleotide polymorphism A118G of the OPRM, such that normal human subjects with the variant allele have been shown to require almost twice as high a plasma level of morphine to achieve the analgesic response as those with the nonmutated allele (253,254,259). Other genes that have been linked to pain and opioid responses include those that code for the delta opioid receptor (260), the capsaicin- sensing vanilloid receptor (260), the neurotransmitter enzyme catechol-O-methyltransferase (260–262), and the melanocortin-1 receptor gene (263).
Genetics and Opioid-Induced Hyperalgesia (OIH)
Genetic differences in response to opioids influence the propensity to develop OIH. In a series of studies, Kest et al. reported murine strain differences in the development of opioid tolerance (264) and withdrawal severity (265,266), both of which have been ascribed a role in the presentation of OIH. Building upon preliminary evidence for genetic differences in OIH development (146), Liang et al. (267) evaluated 16 different strains of inbred mice for the development of thermal hyperalgesia following 4 days of morphine pretreatment and found significant variation among strains. Percent reduction in nociceptive thresholds ranged from 4% (LP/J strain) to 36% (AJR/J strain) in these experiments, and interestingly, a strain found to be relatively pain intolerant (C57BL/6J) in previous work also developed a notable degree of hyperalgesia (24%) following chronic morphine administration.
In a related study, this investigative group evaluated the effects of chronic inflammatory pain on thermal pain perception and opioid responses across strains of mice (268). Despite considerable variation, chronic pain induced thermal hyperalgesia and increased sensitivity to morphine analgesia across animals tested. Further, following 4 days of opioid administration, those both with and without pain demonstrated dependence and tolerance, albeit with significant strain variation. Suggesting a shared mechanism for OIH, opioid tolerance, and dependence, haplotypic genetic analyses revealed that differential expression of a gene coding for the nonspecific P-glycoprotein transporter (Abcb1b gene) best accounted for strain-related differences in the development of thermal hyperalgesia following morphine administration (267). Activity of this glycoprotein, which is a transporter for morphine, appears to play an important role in opioid response across domains.
CONCLUSIONS
Pain is the most modulated of the sensory modalities. How a given, quantifiable stimulus is processed by the nervous system can be modified at the level of the nociceptor, the peripheral nerve, the spinal cord neurons and tracts, the spinal cord microglia, and the thalamus and its interactions with the cortex and various activating centers such as the amygdala. Modulation typically occurs as a dynamic interaction of processes among these sites. The unique susceptibility of pain to neuromodulation and the reversal of neuroplastic changes portend a significant role for addiction in altering the pain experience. Addiction physiology underlies the pathophysiologic processing of the motivational system encoding reward and stress/antireward; stimuli that are by nature stressful and unrewarding, such as pain, are likely to be preferentially affected by the presence of addiction.
Much of the evidence for overlap between chronic pain and addiction phenomena has been obtained in animal studies, so caution must be exercised in generalizing these to clinical populations. Both pain and addiction are uniquely expressed in humans; the complexity of each is evident in psychosocial, cognitive, and cultural domains and cannot be completely replicated in animal models. Hypotheses about how the neurophysiologic overlap between chronic pain and addiction might clinically manifest must take into account how holistic human responses to their combined presence may alter or mask predicted physiologic responses.
Several points of overlap exist between the physiologic bases of pain and addiction. Specific avenues by which the chronic use of addictive drugs might alter the processing of noxious stimuli include the presence of sympathetic stimulation, HPA axis dysregulation, affective withdrawal, and opioid tolerance. Across this literature, a trend toward decreased pain tolerance in addiction can be discerned; thus, the presence of addictive disease appears to augment the experience of pain.
With respect to opioid addiction specifically, the development or presence of OIH has the potential to complicate pain management. Less well studied but intriguing is evidence that individuals vary in their propensity to both addiction and pain responses; the link between the two may arise from inborn differences in endogenous opioid system tone and/or systems in place to maintain endogenous opioid tone (opponent processes).
Consideration of the physiologic bases of pain and addiction, and how they overlap, provides direction for the management of pain in this population. The human phenomena of pain and addiction are not separate but interrelated; knowledgeable management of the former must reflect the extent to which, even at the physiologic level, its expression and response are affected by the latter (4,11–12,18,22,85–88,230,269–271).
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The authors wish to recognize the contributions of Peggy Compton, PhD, RN, to the preparation of this chapter.
REFERENCES
1.Dubois MY, Gallagher RM, Lippe PM. Pain medicine position paper. Pain Med 2009;10(6):972–1000.
2.IOM. Relieving pain in America: a blueprint for transforming prevention, carse, education, and research. http://www.iom.edu/Reports/2011/Relieving-Pain-in-America-A-Blueprint-for-Transforming-Prevention-Care-Education-Research.aspx. June 29, 2011. Accessed May 11, 2013.
3.Gallagher RM. Pain science and rational polypharmacy: an historical perspective. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2005;84(3 Suppl):S1–S3.
4.Gallagher RM. Treatment planning in pain medicine. Integrating medical, physical, and behavioral therapies. Med Clin North Am 1999;83(3):823–849, viii.
5.Gallagher RM. The pain medicine and primary care community rehabilitation model: monitored care for pain disorders in multiple settings. Clin J Pain 1999;15(1):1–3.
6.Schatman ME. The role of the health insurance industry in perpetuating suboptimal pain management. Pain Med 2011;12(3):415–426.
7.Meghani SH. Corporatization of pain medicine: implications for widening pain care disparities. Pain Med 2011;12(4):634–644.
8.Gallagher RM. Chronification to maldynia: biopsychosocial failure of pain homeostasis. Pain Med 2011;12(7):993–995.
9.Acute Pain—Pathophysiology and Risk Factors for Chronification. Benzon Symposium No. 57, October 4–7, 2010, Copenhagen, Denmark. Available at: http://www.benzonsymposia.dk/symp_57_synopsis.html. Accessed June 15, 2011.
10.Bowering KJ, O’Connell NE, Tabor A, et al. The effects of graded motor imagery and its components on chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain 2013;14(1):3–13.
11.Vachon-Presseau E, Martel MO, Roy M, et al. Acute stress contributes to individual differences in pain and pain-related brain activity in healthy and chronic pain patients. J Neurosci2013;33(16):6826–6833.
12.Vachon-Presseau E, Roy M, Martel MO, et al. The stress model of chronic pain: evidence from basal cortisol and hippocampal structure and function in humans. Brain 2013;136(Pt 3):815–827.
13.Milligan ED, Watkins LR. Pathological and protective roles of glia in chronic pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10(1):23–36.
14.Loram LC, Taylor FR, Strand KA, et al. Prior exposure to glucocorticoids potentiates lipopolysaccharide induced mechanical allodynia and spinal neuroinflammation. Brain Behav Immun2011;25(7):1408–1415.
15.Melzack R, Wall PD. Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science 1965;150(3699):971–979.
16.Moseley GL. Graded motor imagery for pathologic pain: a randomized controlled trial. Neurology 2006;67(12):2129–2134.
17.deCharms RC, Maeda F, Glover GH, et al. Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005;102(51):18626–18631.
18.Gallagher RM. Biopsychosocial pain medicine and mind-brain-body science. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am 2004;15(4):855–882, vii.
19.Gallagher RM. Rational integration of pharmacologic, behavioral, and rehabilitation strategies in the treatment of chronic pain. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2005;84(3 Suppl):S64–S76.
20.Gallagher RM. Management of neuropathic pain: translating mechanistic advances and evidence-based research into clinical practice. Clin J Pain 2006;22(1 Suppl):S2–S8.
21.Gallagher RM. Analgesic selection in the management of chronic pain: linking mechanisms & evidence-based research to clinical practice. Clin J Pain 2006;22(1 Suppl):S1.
22.Craig AD. A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends Neurosci 2003;26(6):303–307.
23.Cutler RB, Fishbain DA, Rosomoff HL, et al. Does nonsurgical pain center treatment of chronic pain return patients to work? A review and meta-analysis of the literature. Spine (Phila Pa 1976)1994;19(6):643–652.
24.Lang E, Liebig K, Kastner S, et al. Multidisciplinary rehabilitation versus usual care for chronic low back pain in the community: effects on quality of life. Spine J 2003;3(4):270–276.
25.Guzman J, Esmail R, Karjalainen K, et al. Multidisciplinary rehabilitation for chronic low back pain: systematic review. BMJ 2001;322(7301):1511–1516.
26.Guzman J, Esmail R, Karjalainen K, et al. Multidisciplinary biopsychosocial rehabilitation for chronic low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2002;(1):CD000963.
27.Cervero F, Laird JM. Mechanisms of touch-evoked pain (allodynia): a new model. Pain 1996;68(1):13–23.
28.Cervero F, Laird JM. Mechanisms of allodynia: interactions between sensitive mechanoreceptors and nociceptors. Neuroreport 1996;7(2):526–528.
29.Mendell JR, Sahenk Z. Clinical practice. Painful sensory neuropathy. N Engl J Med 2003;348(13):1243–1255.
30.Schaible HG, Schmidt RF. Activation of groups III and IV sensory units in medial articular nerve by local mechanical stimulation of knee joint. J Neurophysiol 1983;49(1):35–44.
31.Tal M, Bennett GJ. Extra-territorial pain in rats with a peripheral mononeuropathy: mechano-hyperalgesia and mechano-allodynia in the territory of an uninjured nerve. Pain1994;57(3):375–382.
32.Sandkuler J. Models and mechanisms of hyperalgesia and allodynia. Physiol Rev 2009;89(2):707–758.
33.Willis WD, Westlund KN. Neuroanatomy of the pain system and of the pathways that modulate pain. J Clin Neurophysiol 1997;14(1):2–31.
34.O’Connell PJ, Pingle SC, Ahern GP. Dendritic cells do not transduce inflammatory stimuli via the capsaicin receptor TRPV1. FEBS Lett 2005;579(23):5135–5139.
35.Fields HL. Pain: an unpleasant topic. Pain 1999;Suppl 6:S61–S69.
36.Woolf CJ, Mannion RJ. Neuropathic pain: aetiology, symptoms, mechanisms, and management. Lancet 1999;353(9168):1959–1964.
37.Dickinson BD, Head CA, Gitlow S, et al. Maldynia: pathophysiology and management of neuropathic and maladaptive pain—a report of the AMA Council on Science and Public Health. Pain Med2010;11(11):1635–1653.
38.Rome HP Jr, Rome JD. Limbically augmented pain syndrome (LAPS): kindling, corticolimbic sensitization, and the convergence of affective and sensory symptoms in chronic pain disorders. Pain Med2000;1(1):7–23.
39.Attal N, Bouhassira D. Mechanisms of pain in peripheral neuropathy. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 1999;173:12–24; discussion 48–52.
40.Abraham WC, Robins A. Memory retention—the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma. Trends Neurosci 2005;28(2):73–78.
41.Piche M, Arsenault M, Rainville P. Cerebral and cerebrospinal processes underlying counterirritation analgesia. J Neurosci 2009;29(45):14236–14246.
42.Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, et al. Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science 1997;277(5328):968–971.
43.Rainville P, Carrier B, Hofbauer RK, et al. Dissociation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain using hypnotic modulation. Pain 1999;82(2):159–171.
44.Vogt BA. Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005;6(7):533–544.
45.Burgmer M, Pfleiderer B, Maihofner C, et al. Cerebral mechanisms of experimental hyperalgesia in fibromyalgia. Eur J Pain;16(5):636–647.
46.Iles RA, Davidson M, Taylor NF. Psychosocial predictors of failure to return to work in non-chronic non-specific low back pain: a systematic review. Occup Environ Med 2008;65(8):507–517.
47.Gallagher RM, Rauh V, Haugh LD, et al. Determinants of return-to-work among low back pain patients. Pain 1989;39(1):55–67.
48.Vowles KE, McCracken LM, Eccleston C. Patient functioning and catastrophizing in chronic pain: the mediating effects of acceptance. Health Psychol 2008;27(2 Suppl):S136–S143.
49.Vowles KE, McCracken LM, Eccleston C. Processes of change in treatment for chronic pain: the contributions of pain, acceptance, and catastrophizing. Eur J Pain 2007;11(7):779–787.
50.McWilliams LA, Cox BJ, Enns MW. Mood and anxiety disorders associated with chronic pain: an examination in a nationally representative sample. Pain 2003;106(1–2):127–133.
51.Gallagher RM, Verma S. Mood and anxiety disorders in chronic pain. In: Dworkin R, Brieghtbart W, eds. Psychosocial and psychiatric aspects of pain: a handbook for health care providers. Progress in pain research and management, Vol. 27. Seattle, WA: IASP Press, 2004.
52.Gureje O, Von Korff M, Simon GE, et al. Persistent pain and well-being: a World Health Organization study in primary care. JAMA 1998;280(2):147–151.
53.Miller GE, Rohleder N, Stetler C, et al. Clinical depression and regulation of the inflammatory response during acute stress. Psychosom Med 2005;67(5):679–687.
54.Gallagher RM, Marbach JJ, Raphael KG, et al. Is major depression comorbid with temporomandibular pain and dysfunction syndrome? A pilot study. Clin J Pain 1991;7(3):219–225.
55.Gallagher RM, Marbach J, Raphael K, et al. Seasonal variation in chronic TMPDS pain and mood intensity. Pain 1995; 61(1):113–120.
56.Dohrenwend BP, Raphael KG, Marbach JJ, et al. Why is depression comorbid with chronic myofascial face pain? A family study test of alternative hypotheses. Pain 1999;83(2):183–192.
57.Raphael KG, Janal MN, Nayak S, et al. Familial aggregation of depression in fibromyalgia: a community-based test of alternate hypotheses. Pain 2004;110(1–2):449–460.
58.Arnold LM, Hudson JI, Hess EV, et al. Family study of fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum 2004;50(3):944–952.
59.Russell IJ, Michalek JE, Vipraio GA, et al. Platelet 3H-imipramine uptake receptor density and serum serotonin levels in patients with fibromyalgia/fibrositis syndrome. J Rheumatol1992;19(1):104–109.
60.Russell IJ, Vaeroy H, Javors M, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biogenic amine metabolites in fibromyalgia/fibrositis syndrome and rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 1992;35(5):550–556.
61.Yunus MB, Dailey JW, Aldag JC, et al. Plasma and urinary catecholamines in primary fibromyalgia: a controlled study. J Rheumatol 1992;19(1):95–97.
62.Schwarz MJ, Spath M, Muller-Bardorff H, et al. Relationship of substance P, 5-hydroxyindole acetic acid and tryptophan in serum of fibromyalgia patients. Neurosci Lett 1999;259(3):196–198.
63.Charney DS. Monoamine dysfunction and the pathophysiology and treatment of depression. J Clin Psychiatry 1998;59(Suppl 14):11–14.
64.Hirschfeld RM. History and evolution of the monoamine hypothesis of depression. J Clin Psychiatry 2000;61(Suppl 6):4–6.
65.Bondy B, Spaeth M, Offenbaecher M, et al. The T102C polymorphism of the 5-HT2A-receptor gene in fibromyalgia. Neurobiol Dis 1999;6(5):433–439.
66.Cohen H, Buskila D, Neumann L, et al. Confirmation of an association between fibromyalgia and serotonin transporter promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism, and relationship to anxiety-related personality traits. Arthritis Rheum 2002;46(3):845–847.
67.Gursoy S. Absence of association of the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism with the mentally healthy subset of fibromyalgia patients. Clin Rheumatol 2002;21(3):194–197.
68.Offenbaecher M, Bondy B, de Jonge S, et al. Possible association of fibromyalgia with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Arthritis Rheum1999;42(11):2482–2488.
69.Kendler KS, Kessler RC, Neale MC, et al. The prediction of major depression in women: toward an integrated etiologic model. Am J Psychiatry 1993;150(8):1139–1148.
70.Riso LP, Miyatake RK, Thase ME. The search for determinants of chronic depression: a review of six factors. J Affect Disord 2002;70(2):103–115.
71.Shrout PE, Link BG, Dohrenwend BP, et al. Characterizing life events as risk factors for depression: the role of fateful loss events. J Abnorm Psychol 1989;98(4):460–467.
72.Tennant C. Life events, stress and depression: a review of recent findings. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2002;36(2):173–182.
73.Raphael KG, Natelson BH, Janal MN, et al. A community-based survey of fibromyalgia-like pain complaints following the World Trade Center terrorist attacks. Pain 2002;100(1–2):131–139.
74.Davis MC, Zautra AJ, Reich JW. Vulnerability to stress among women in chronic pain from fibromyalgia and osteoarthritis. Ann Behav Med 2001;23(3):215–226.
75.Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 2003;301(5631):386–389.
76.Moseley GL. Is successful rehabilitation of complex regional pain syndrome due to sustained attention to the affected limb? A randomised clinical trial. Pain 2005;114(1–2):54–61.
77.Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, et al. MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160(5):924–932.
78.Mohanakrishnan Menon P, Nasrallah HA, Lyons JA, et al. Single-voxel proton MR spectroscopy of right versus left hippocampi in PTSD. Psychiatry Res 2003;123(2):101–108.
79.Lindauer RJ, Vlieger EJ, Jalink M, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume in Dutch police officers with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2004;56(5):356–363.
80.Hedges DW, Farrer TJ, Brown BL. Association between C-reactive protein and cognitive deficits in elderly men and women: a meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr 2012;24(9):1387–1392.
81.Hadjikhani N, de Gelder B. Seeing fearful body expressions activates the fusiform cortex and amygdala. Curr Biol 2003;13(24): 2201–2205.
82.Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, et al. Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biol Psychiatry 2003;53(6):494–501.
83.Morris JS, Dolan RJ. Dissociable amygdala and orbitofrontal responses during reversal fear conditioning. Neuroimage 2004;22(1):372–380.
84.Grachev ID, Fredrickson BE, Apkarian AV. Brain chemistry reflects dual states of pain and anxiety in chronic low back pain. J Neural Transm 2002;109(10):1309–1334.
85.Baliki MN, Chialvo DR, Geha PY, et al. Chronic pain and the emotional brain: specific brain activity associated with spontaneous fluctuations of intensity of chronic back pain. J Neurosci2006;26(47):12165–12173.
86.Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science 1997;278(5335):52–58.
87.Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;35:217–238.
88.O’Brien CP. Drug addiction and drug abuse. In: Brunton LL, Lazo JS, Parker KL, eds. Goodman and Gilman’s pharmacological basis of therapeutics, 11th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, 2006:607–625.
89.Koob GF, Bloom FE. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. Science 1988;242(4879):715–723.
90.Basbaum AI. Insights into the development of opioid tolerance. Pain 1995;61(3):349–352.
91.Solomon RL. The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation: the costs of pleasure and the benefits of pain. Am Psychol 1980;35(8):691–712.
92.Koob GF, Stinus L, Le Moal M, et al. Opponent process theory of motivation: neurobiological evidence from studies of opiate dependence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1989;13(2–3):135–140.
93.Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;24(2):97–129.
94.Koob GF, Le Moal M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu Rev Psychol 2008;59:29–53.
95.Christie MJ. Cellular neuroadaptations to chronic opioids: tolerance, withdrawal and addiction. Br J Pharmacol 2008;154(2):384–396.
96.Koch T, Hollt V. Role of receptor internalization in opioid tolerance and dependence. Pharmacol Ther. 2008;117(2):199–206.
97.Chao J, Nestler EJ. Molecular neurobiology of drug addiction. Annu Rev Med 2004;55:113–132.
98.Finn AK, Whistler JL. Endocytosis of the mu opioid receptor reduces tolerance and a cellular hallmark of opiate withdrawal. Neuron 2001;32(5):829–839.
99.Bailey CP, Connor M. Opioids: cellular mechanisms of tolerance and physical dependence. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2005;5(1):60–68.
100.Redmond DE Jr, Krystal JH. Multiple mechanisms of withdrawal from opioid drugs. Annu Rev Neurosci 1984;7:443–478.
101.Gold JM, Weinberger DR. Cognitive deficits and the neurobiology of schizophrenia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1995;5(2):225–230.
102.Koob GF. Neuroadaptive mechanisms of addiction: studies on the extended amygdala. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2003;13(6): 442–452.
103.Aston-Jones G, Delfs JM, Druhan J, et al. The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis: a target site for noradrenergic actions in opiate withdrawal. In: McGinty JF, ed. Advancing from the Ventral Striatum to the Extended Amygdala: Implications for Neuropsychiatry and Drug Abuse (series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 877). New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences, 1999: 486–498.
104.Taricco M, Pagliacci MC, Telaro E, et al. Pharmacological interventions for spasticity following spinal cord injury: results of a Cochrane systematic review. Eura Medicophys2006;42(1):5–15.
105.Bahn EL, Holt KR. Procedural sedation and analgesia: a review and new concepts. Emerg Med Clin North Am 2005;23(2):503–517.
106.Mazurek MS. Sedation and analgesia for procedures outside the operating room. Semin Pediatr Surg 2004;13(3):166–173.
107.Devane WA, Hanus L, Breuer A, et al. Isolation and structure of a brain constituent that binds to the cannabinoid receptor. Science 1992;258(5090):1946–1949.
108.Herkenham M. In: Pertwee RG, ed. Cannabinoid receptors. New York, NY: Academic Press, 1995:145–166.
109.Matsuda LA, Lolait SJ, Brownstein MJ, et al. Structure of a cannabinoid receptor and functional expression of the cloned cDNA. Nature 1990;346(6284):561–564.
110.Mao J, Price DD, Lu J, et al. Two distinctive antinociceptive systems in rats with pathological pain. Neurosci Lett 2000;280(1):13–16.
111.Rog DJ, Nurmikko TJ, Young CA. Oromucosal delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol/cannabidiol for neuropathic pain associated with multiple sclerosis: an uncontrolled, open-label, 2-year extension trial. Clin Ther2007;29(9):2068–2079.
112.Wade DT, Makela P, Robson P, et al. Do cannabis-based medicinal extracts have general or specific effects on symptoms in multiple sclerosis? A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study on 160 patients. Mult Scler 2004;10(4):434–441.
113.Hosking RD, Zajicek JP. Therapeutic potential of cannabis in pain medicine. Br J Anaesth 2008;101(1):59–68.
114.Palazzo E, Marabese I, de Novellis V, et al. Metabotropic and NMDA glutamate receptors participate in the cannabinoid-induced antinociception. Neuropharmacology 2001;40(3):319–326.
115.Palazzos E, de Novellis V, Marabese I, et al. Metabotropic glutamate and cannabinoid receptor crosstalk in periaqueductal grey pain processing. Curr Neuropharmacol 2006;4(3):225–231.
116.Nunes EV, Rounsaville BJ. Comorbidity of substance use with depression and other mental disorders. From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed (DSM-IV) to DSM-5. Addiction2006;101 (Suppl 1):89–96.
117.Schuckit MA. Comorbidity between substance use disorders and psychiatric conditions. Addiction 2006;101 Suppl 1:76–88.
118.Begre S, Traber M, Gerber M, et al. Change in pain severity with open label venlafaxine use in patients with a depressive symptomatology: an observational study in primary care. Eur Psychiatry2008;23(3):178–186.
119.Krebs EE, Gaynes BN, Gartlehner G, et al. Treating the physical symptoms of depression with second-generation antidepressants: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Psychosomatics 2008;49(3):191–198.
120.Perahia DG, Pritchett YL, Desaiah D, et al. Efficacy of duloxetine in painful symptoms: an analgesic or antidepressant effect? Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2006;21(6):311–317.
121.Compton MA. Cold-pressor pain tolerance in opiate and cocaine abusers: correlates of drug type and use status. J Pain Symptom Manage 1994;9(7):462–473.
122.Bruijnzeel AW, Repetto M, Gold MS. Neurobiological mechanisms in addictive and psychiatric disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2004;27(4):661–674.
123.Kita M, Goodkin DE. Drugs used to treat spasticity. Drugs 2000;59(3):487–495.
124.Compton P, Charuvastra VC, Kintaudi K, et al. Pain responses in methadone-maintained opioid abusers. J Pain Symptom Manage 2000;20(4):237–245.
125.Compton P, Charuvastra VC, Ling W. Pain intolerance in opioid-maintained former opiate addicts: effect of long-acting maintenance agent. Drug Alcohol Depend 2001;63(2):139–146.
126.Gutstein HB, Akil H. In: Brunton LL, Lazo JS, Parker KL, eds. Goodman and Gilman’s pharmacological basis of therapeutics, 11th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, 2006:457–590.
127.Koob GF. A role for brain stress systems in addiction. Neuron 2008;59(1):11–34.
128.Shurman J, Koob GF, Gutstein HB. Opioids, pain, the brain, and hyperkatifeia: a framework for the rational use of opioids for pain. Pain Med 2010;11(7):1092–1098.
129.Price D. Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 2000;288:1769–1772.
130.Bester H, Menendez L, Besson JM, et al. Spino (trigemino) parabrachiohypothalamic pathway: electrophysiological evidence for an involvement in pain processes. J Neurophysiol1995;73: 568–585.
131.Neugebauer V, Li W. Processing of nociceptive mechanical and thermal information in central amygdala neurons with knee-joint input, 2002. J Neurophysiol 2002;87:103–112.
132.Roberto M, Gilpin NW, O’Dell LE, et al. Cellular and behavioral interactions of gabapentin with alcohol dependence. J Neurosci 2008;28(22):5762–5771.
133.Funk C, O’Dell L, Crawford E, et al. Corticotropin-releasing factor within the central nucleus of the amygdala mediates enhanced ethanol self-administration in withdrawn, ethanol-dependent rats. J Neurosci2006;26:11324–11332.
134.Edwards S, Vendruscolo LF, Schlosburg JE, et al. Development of mechanical hypersensitivity in rats during heroin and ethanol dependence: alleviation by CRF(1) receptor antagonism. Neuropharmacology 2012;62(2):1142–1151.
135.Albutt C. On the abuse of hypodermic injections of morphia. Practitioner 1870;3:327–330.
136.Martin JE, Inglis J. Pain tolerance and narcotic addiction. Br J Soc Clin Psychol 1965;4(3):224–229.
137.Ho A, Dole VP. Pain perception in drug-free and in methadone-maintained human ex-addicts. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 1979;162(3):392–395.
138.Doverty M, White JM, Somogyi AA, et al. Hyperalgesic responses in methadone maintenance patients. Pain 2001;90(1–2):91–96.
139.Schall U, Katta T, Pries E, et al. Pain perception of intravenous heroin users on maintenance therapy with levomethadone. Pharmacopsychiatry 1996;29(5):176–179.
140.Athanasos P, Smith CS, White JM, et al. Methadone maintenance patients are cross-tolerant to the antinociceptive effects of very high plasma morphine concentrations. Pain2006;120(3):267–275.
141.Pud D, Cohen D, Lawental E, et al. Opioids and abnormal pain perception: New evidence from a study of chronic opioid addicts and healthy subjects. Drug Alcohol Depend2006;82(3):218–223.
142.Angst MS, Koppert W, Pahl I, et al. Short-term infusion of the mu-opioid agonist remifentanil in humans causes hyperalgesia during withdrawal. Pain. Nov 2003;106(1–2):49–57.
143.Angst MS, Clark JD. Opioid-induced hyperalgesia: a qualitative systematic review. Anesthesiology 2006;104(3):570–587.
144.Chu LF, Angst MS, Clark D. Opioid-induced hyperalgesia in humans: molecular mechanisms and clinical considerations. Clin J Pain 2008;24(6):479–496.
145.Koppert W, Angst M, Alsheimer M, et al. Naloxone provokes similar pain facilitation as observed after short-term infusion of remifentanil in humans. Pain 2003;106(1–2):91–99.
146.Li X, Angst MS, Clark JD. A murine model of opioid-induced hyperalgesia. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 2001;86(1–2):56–62.
147.Mao J. Opioid-induced abnormal pain sensitivity: implications in clinical opioid therapy. Pain 2002;100(3):213–217.
148.Simonnet G. Opioids: from analgesia to anti-hyperalgesia? Pain. 2005;118(1–2):8–9.
149.Vanderah TW, Ossipov MH, Lai J, et al. Mechanisms of opioid-induced pain and antinociceptive tolerance: descending facilitation and spinal dynorphin. Pain 2001;92(1–2):5–9.
150.Kaplan H, Fields HL. Hyperalgesia during acute opioid abstinence: evidence for a nociceptive facilitating function of the rostral ventromedial medulla. J Neurosci 1991;11(5):1433–1439.
151.Martin WR, Gilbert PE, Jasinski DR, et al. An analysis of naltrexone precipitated abstinence in morphine-dependent chronic spinal dogs. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1987;240(2):565–570.
152.Sweitzer SM, Allen CP, Zissen MH, et al. Mechanical allodynia and thermal hyperalgesia upon acute opioid withdrawal in the neonatal rat. Pain 2004;110(1–2):269–280.
153.Tilson HA, Rech RH, Stolman S. Hyperalgesia during withdrawal as a means of measuring the degree of dependence in morphine dependent rats. Psychopharmacologia1973;28(3):287–300.
154.Jasinski DS. Assessment of the abuse liability of the morphine-like drugs (methods used in man). In: Martin WR, ed. Handbook of experimental pharmacology, Vol. 45: Drug Addiction I. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, 1997:197–258.
155.Bederson JB, Fields HL, Barbaro NM. Hyperalgesia during naloxone-precipitated withdrawal from morphine is associated with increased on-cell activity in the rostral ventromedial medulla. Somatosens Mot Res 1990;7(2):185–203.
156.Grilly DM, Gowans GC. Acute morphine dependence: effects observed in shock and light discrimination tasks. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1986;88(4):500–504.
157.Kim DH, Fields HL, Barbaro NM. Morphine analgesia and acute physical dependence: rapid onset of two opposing, dose-related processes. Brain Res 1990;516(1):37–40.
158.Holtman JR Jr, Wala EP. Characterization of morphine-induced hyperalgesia in male and female rats. Pain 2005;114(1–2):62–70.
159.Chu LF, Clark DJ, Angst MS. Opioid tolerance and hyperalgesia in chronic pain patients after one month of oral morphine therapy: a preliminary prospective study. J Pain 2006;7(1):43–48.
160.Ossipov MH, Lai J, King T, et al. Underlying mechanisms of pronociceptive consequences of prolonged morphine exposure. Biopolymers 2005;80(2–3):319–324.
161.Celerier E, Rivat C, Jun Y, et al. Long-lasting hyperalgesia induced by fentanyl in rats: preventive effect of ketamine. Anesthesiology 2000;92(2):465–472.
162.Gardell LR, King T, Ossipov MH, et al. Opioid receptor-mediated hyperalgesia and antinociceptive tolerance induced by sustained opiate delivery. Neurosci Lett 2006;396(1):44–49.
163.Ibuki T, Dunbar SA, Yaksh TL. Effect of transient naloxone antagonism on tolerance development in rats receiving continuous spinal morphine infusion. Pain 1997;70(2–3):125–132.
164.Van Elstraete AC, Sitbon P, Trabold F, et al. A single dose of intrathecal morphine in rats induces long-lasting hyperalgesia: the protective effect of prior administration of ketamine. Anesth Analg2005;101(6):1750–1756.
165.Compton P, Athanasos P, Elashoff D. Withdrawal hyperalgesia after acute opioid physical dependence in nonaddicted humans: a preliminary study. J Pain 2003;4:511–519.
166.Bie B, Pan ZZ. Presynaptic mechanism for anti-analgesic and anti-hyperalgesic actions of opioid receptors. J Neurosci 2003;23:7262–7268.
167.Ren Z-Y, Shi J, Epstein DH, et al. Abnormal pain response in pain sensitive opiate addicts after prolonged abstinence predicts increased drug craving. Psychopharmacology (Berl)2009;204:423–429.
168.Jasinski DR. Tolerance and dependence to opiates. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1997;41(1 Pt 2):184–186.
169.Colpaert FC. System theory of pain and of opiate analgesia: no tolerance to opiates. Pharmacol Rev 1996;48(3):355–402.
170.Colpaert FC. Mechanisms of opioid-induced pain and antinociceptive tolerance: signal transduction. Pain 2002;95(3):287–288.
171.Celerier E, Laulin J, Larcher A, et al. Evidence for opiate-activated NMDA processes masking opiate analgesia in rats. Brain Res 1999;847(1):18–25.
172.Celerier E, Laulin JP, Corcuff JB, et al. Progressive enhancement of delayed hyperalgesia induced by repeated heroin administration: a sensitization process. J Neurosci2001;21(11):4074–4080.
173.Laulin JP, Celerier E, Larcher A, et al. Opiate tolerance to daily heroin administration: an apparent phenomenon associated with enhanced pain sensitivity. Neuroscience1999;89(3):631–636.
174.Finnerup NB, Otto M, McQuay HJ, et al. Algorithm for neuropathic pain treatment: an evidence based proposal. Pain 2005;118(3):289–305.
175.Foley KM. Opioids and chronic neuropathic pain. N Engl J Med 2003;348(13):1279–1281.
176.Mao J, Price DD, Mayer DJ. Thermal hyperalgesia in association with the development of morphine tolerance in rats: roles of excitatory amino acid receptors and protein kinase C. J Neurosci1994;14(4):2301–2312.
177.Mao J, Price DD, Mayer DJ. Experimental mononeuropathy reduces the antinociceptive effects of morphine: implications for common intracellular mechanisms involved in morphine tolerance and neuropathic pain. Pain 1995;61(3):353–364.
178.Mao J, Price DD, Mayer DJ. Mechanisms of hyperalgesia and morphine tolerance: a current view of their possible interactions. Pain 1995;62(3):259–274.
179.Mayer DJ, Mao J, Price DD. The association of neuropathic pain, morphine tolerance and dependence, and the translocation of protein kinase C. NIDA Res Monogr 1995;147:269–298.
180.Mayer DJ, Mao J, Price DD. The development of morphine tolerance and dependence is associated with translocation of protein kinase C. Pain 1995;61(3):365–374.
181.Dogrul A, Bilsky EJ, Ossipov MH, et al. Spinal L-type calcium channel blockade abolishes opioid-induced sensory hypersensitivity and antinociceptive tolerance. Anesth Analg2005;101(6):1730–1735.
182.Gardell LR, Wang R, Burgess SE, et al. Sustained morphine exposure induces a spinal dynorphin-dependent enhancement of excitatory transmitter release from primary afferent fibers. J Neurosci2002;22(15):6747–6755.
183.King T, Gardell LR, Wang R, et al. Role of NK-1 neurotransmission in opioid-induced hyperalgesia. Pain 2005;116(3):276–288.
184.Lim G, Wang S, Zeng Q, et al. Evidence for a long-term influence on morphine tolerance after previous morphine exposure: role of neuronal glucocorticoid receptors. Pain 2005;114(1–2):81–92.
185.Raghavendra V, Rutkowski MD, DeLeo JA. The role of spinal neuroimmune activation in morphine tolerance/ hyperalgesia in neuropathic and sham-operated rats. J Neurosci2002;22(22):9980–9989.
186.Vanderah TW, Gardell LR, Burgess SE, et al. Dynorphin promotes abnormal pain and spinal opioid antinociceptive tolerance. J Neurosci 2000;20(18):7074–7079.
187.Vanderah TW, Suenaga NM, Ossipov MH, et al. Tonic descending facilitation from the rostral ventromedial medulla mediates opioid-induced abnormal pain and antinociceptive tolerance. J Neurosci2001;21(1):279–286.
188.Mayer DJ, Mao J, Holt J, et al. Cellular mechanisms of neuropathic pain, morphine tolerance, and their interactions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999;96(14):7731–7736.
189.Dunbar SA, Pulai IJ. Repetitive opioid abstinence causes progressive hyperalgesia sensitive to N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther1998;284(2):678–686.
190.Dunbar S, Yaksh TL. Concurrent spinal infusion of MK801 blocks spinal tolerance and dependence induced by chronic intrathecal morphine in the rat. Anesthesiology 1996;84(5):1177–1188.
191.Larcher A, Laulin JP, Celerier E, et al. Acute tolerance associated with a single opiate administration: involvement of N-methyl-D-aspartate-dependent pain facilitatory systems. Neuroscience1998;84(2):583–589.
192.Richebe P, Rivat C, Creton C, et al. Nitrous oxide revisited: evidence for potent antihyperalgesic properties. Anesthesiology 2005;103(4):845–854.
193.Portenoy RK, Bennett GJ, Katz NP, et al. Enhancing opioid analgesia with NMDA-receptor antagonists: clarifying the clinical importance. A roundtable discussion. J Pain Symptom Manage 2000;19(1 Suppl): S57–S64.
194.Price DD, Mayer DJ, Mao J, et al. NMDA-receptor antagonists and opioid receptor interactions as related to analgesia and tolerance. J Pain Symptom Manage 2000;19(1 Suppl):S7–S11.
195.Sang CN. NMDA-receptor antagonists in neuropathic pain: experimental methods to clinical trials. J Pain Symptom Manage 2000;19(1 Suppl):S21–S25.
196.Weinbroum AA, Rudick V, Paret G, et al. The role of dextromethorphan in pain control. Can J Anaesth 2000;47(6):585–596.
197.Bisaga A, Popik P. In search of a new pharmacological treatment for drug and alcohol addiction: N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) antagonists. Drug Alcohol Depend 2000;59(1):1–15.
198.Elliott K, Hynansky A, Inturrisi CE. Dextromethorphan attenuates and reverses analgesic tolerance to morphine. Pain 1994;59(3):361–368.
199.Pasternak GW, Kolesnikov YA, Babey AM. Perspectives on the N-methyl-D-aspartate/nitric oxide cascade and opioid tolerance. Neuropsychopharmacology 1995;13(4):309–313.
200.Trujillo KA. Effects of noncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists on opiate tolerance and physical dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology 1995;13(4):301–307.
201.Compton PA, Ling W, Torrington MA. Lack of effect of chronic dextromethorphan on experimental pain tolerance in methadone-maintained patients. Addict Biol 2008;13(3–4):393–402.
202.Ossipov MH, Lai J, King T, et al. Antinociceptive and nociceptive actions of opioids. J Neurobiol 2004;61(1):126–148.
203.Xie JY, Herman DS, Stiller CO, et al. Cholecystokinin in the rostral ventromedial medulla mediates opioid-induced hyperalgesia and antinociceptive tolerance. J Neurosci2005;25(2):409–416.
204.Devillers JP, Boisserie F, Laulin JP, et al. Simultaneous activation of spinal antiopioid system (neuropeptide FF) and pain facilitatory circuitry by stimulation of opioid receptors in rats. Brain Res 1995;700(1–2):173–181.
205.DeLeo JA, Tanga FY, Tawfik VL. Neuroimmune activation and neuroinflammation in chronic pain and opioid tolerance/ hyperalgesia. Neuroscientist 2004;10(1):40–52.
206.Watkins LR, Maier SF. The pain of being sick: implications of immune-to-brain communication for understanding pain. Annu Rev Psychol 2000;51:29–57.
207.Wieseler-Frank J, Maier SF, Watkins LR. Immune-to-brain communication dynamically modulates pain: physiological and pathological consequences. Brain Behav Immun2005;19(2):104–111.
208.Song P, Zhao ZQ. The involvement of glial cells in the development of morphine tolerance. Neurosci Res 2001;39(3):281–286.
209.Johnston IN, Westbrook RF. Inhibition of morphine analgesia by LPS: role of opioid and NMDA receptors and spinal glia. Behav Brain Res 2005;156(1):75–83.
210.Johnston IN, Milligan ED, Wieseler-Frank J, et al. A role for proinflammatory cytokines and fractalkine in analgesia, tolerance, and subsequent pain facilitation induced by chronic intrathecal morphine. J Neurosci 2004;24(33):7353–7365.
211.Hendrie CA. Naloxone-sensitive hyperalgesia follows analgesia induced by morphine and environmental stimulation. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1989;32(4):961–966.
212.McNally GP. Pain facilitatory circuits in the mammalian central nervous system: their behavioral significance and role in morphine analgesic tolerance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev1999;23(8):1059–1078.
213.Krank MD, Hinson RE, Siegel S. Conditional hyperalgesia is elicited by environmental signals of morphine. Behav Neural Biol 1981;32(2):148–157.
214.Siegel S, Hinson RE, Krank MD. The role of predrug signals in morphine analgesic tolerance: support for a Pavlovian conditioning model of tolerance. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1978;4(2):188–196.
215.Chia YY, Liu K, Wang JJ, et al. Intraoperative high dose fentanyl induces postoperative fentanyl tolerance. Can J Anaesth 1999;46(9):872–877.
216.Cooper DW, Lindsay SL, Ryall DM, et al. Does intrathecal fentanyl produce acute cross-tolerance to i.v. morphine? Br J Anaesth 1997;78(3):311–313.
217.Hansen EG, Duedahl TH, Romsing J, et al. Intra-operative remifentanil might influence pain levels in the immediate postoperative period after major abdominal surgery. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand2005;49(10):1464–1470.
218.Guignard B, Bossard AE, Coste C, et al. Acute opioid tolerance: intraoperative remifentanil increases postoperative pain and morphine requirement. Anesthesiology 2000;93(2):409–417.
219.Ali NM. Hyperalgesic response in a patient receiving high concentrations of spinal morphine. Anesthesiology 1986;65(4):449.
220.Mercadante S, Ferrera P, Villari P, et al. Hyperalgesia: an emerging iatrogenic syndrome. J Pain Symptom Manage 2003;26(2):769–775.
221.Mercadante S, Arcuri E. Hyperalgesia and opioid switching. Am J Hosp Palliat Care 2005;22(4):291–294.
222.Sjogren P, Jonsson T, Jensen NH, et al. Hyperalgesia and myoclonus in terminal cancer patients treated with continuous intravenous morphine. Pain 1993;55(1):93–97.
223.Okon TR, George ML. Fentanyl-induced neurotoxicity and paradoxic pain. J Pain Symptom Manage 2008;35(3):327–333.
224.Devulder J. Hyperalgesia induced by high-dose intrathecal sufentanil in neuropathic pain. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol 1997;9(2):146–148.
225.Chou R, Huffman LH. Medications for acute and chronic low back pain: a review of the evidence for an American Pain Society/ American College of Physicians clinical practice guideline. Ann Intern Med2007;147(7):505–514.
226.Eisenberg E, McNicol E, Carr DB. Opioids for neuropathic pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2006(3):CD006146.
227.Furlan AD, Sandoval JA, Mailis-Gagnon A, et al. Opioids for chronic noncancer pain: a meta-analysis of effectiveness and side effects. CMAJ 2006;174(11):1589–1594.
228.Martell BA, O’Connor PG, Kerns RD, et al. Systematic review: opioid treatment for chronic back pain: prevalence, efficacy, and association with addiction. Ann Intern Med2007;146(2):116–127.
229.Kalso E, Edwards JE, Moore RA, et al. Opioids in chronic non-cancer pain: systematic review of efficacy and safety. Pain 2004;112(3):372–380.
230.Fields HL. Should we be reluctant to prescribe opioids for chronic non-malignant pain? Pain 2007;129(3):233–234.
231.Caldwell JR, Hale ME, Boyd RE, et al. Treatment of osteoarthritis pain with controlled release oxycodone or fixed combination oxycodone plus acetaminophen added to nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drugs: a double blind, randomized, multicenter, placebo controlled trial. J Rheumatol 1999;26(4):862–869.
232.Moulin DE, Iezzi A, Amireh R, et al. Randomised trial of oral morphine for chronic non-cancer pain. Lancet 1996;347(8995):143–147.
233.Moulin DE, Palma D, Watling C, et al. Methadone in the management of intractable neuropathic noncancer pain. Can J Neurol Sci 2005;32(3):340–343.
234.Ballantyne JC. Opioids for chronic pain: taking stock. Pain 2006;125(1–2):3–4.
235.Bertilsson L, Dahl ML, Dalen P, et al. Molecular genetics of CYP2D6: clinical relevance with focus on psychotropic drugs. Br J Clin Pharmacol 2002;53(2):111–122.
236.Ingelman-Sundberg M, Johansson I, Persson I, et al. Genetic polymorphism of cytochrome P450. Functional consequences and possible relationship to disease and alcohol toxicity. EXS 1994;71:197–207.
237.Lotsch J, Skarke C, Liefhold J, et al. Genetic predictors of the clinical response to opioid analgesics: clinical utility and future perspectives. Clin Pharmacokinet 2004;43(14):983–1013.
238.Maurer PM, Bartkowski RR. Drug interactions of clinical significance with opioid analgesics. Drug Saf 1993;8(1):30–48.
239.Otton SV, Schadel M, Cheung SW, et al. CYP2D6 phenotype determines the metabolic conversion of hydrocodone to hydromorphone. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1993;54(5):463–472.
240.Smith HS. Opioid metabolism. Mayo Clin Porc 2009;84(7):613–624.
241.Oertel B, Lotsch J. Genetic mutations that prevent pain: implications for future pain medication. Pharmacogenomics 2008;9(2):179–194.
242.Brosen K, Hansen JG, Nielsen KK, et al. Inhibition by paroxetine of desipramine metabolism in extensive but not in poor metabolizers of sparteine. Eur J Clin Pharmacol1993;44(4):349–355.
243.Liebman PM, Lehofer M, Schonauer-Cejpek M, et al. Pain sensitivity in former opioid addicts. Lancet 1994;344(8928):1031–1032.
244.Mogil JS, Marek P, Flodman P, et al. One or two genetic loci mediate high opiate analgesia in selectively bred mice. Pain 1995;60(2):125–135.
245.Mogil JS, Wilson SG, Bon K, et al. Heritability of nociception I: responses of 11 inbred mouse strains on 12 measures of nociception. Pain 1999;80(1–2):67–82.
246.Elmer GI, Pieper JO, Goldberg SR, et al. Opioid operant self-administration, analgesia, stimulation and respiratory depression in mu-deficient mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl)1995;117(1):23–31.
247.Sudakov SK, Goldberg SR, Borisova EV, et al. Differences in morphine reinforcement property in two inbred rat strains: associations with cortical receptors, behavioral activity, analgesia and the cataleptic effects of morphine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1993;112(2–3):183–188.
248.Elmer GI, Pieper JO, Negus SS, et al. Genetic variance in nociception and its relationship to the potency of morphine-induced analgesia in thermal and chemical tests. Pain1998;75(1):129–140.
249.Belknap JK, Mogil JS, Helms ML, et al. Localization to chromosome 10 of a locus influencing morphine analgesia in crosses derived from C57BL/6 and DBA/2 strains. Life Sci1995;57(10):PL117–PL124.
250.Semenova S, Kuzmin A, Zvartau E. Strain differences in the analgesic and reinforcing action of morphine in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1995;50(1):17–21.
251.Berrettini WH, Alexander R, Ferraro TN, et al. A study of oral morphine preference in inbred mouse strains. Psychiatr Genet 1994;4(2):81–86.
252.Petruzzi R, Ferraro TN, Kurschner VC, et al. The effects of repeated morphine exposure on mu opioid receptor number and affinity in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice. Life Sci1997;61(20):2057–2064.
253.Edwards RR. Genetic predictors of acute and chronic pain. Curr Rheumatol Rep 2006;8(6):411–417.
254.Mogil JS, Yu L, Basbaum AI. Pain genes?: natural variation and transgenic mutants. Annu Rev Neurosci 2000;23:777–811.
255.Stamer UM, Stuber F. Genetic factors in pain and its treatment. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol 2007;20(5):478–484.
256.Flores CM, Mogil JS. The pharmacogenetics of analgesia: toward a genetically-based approach to pain management. Pharmacogenomics 2001;2(3):177–194.
257.Mayer P, Hollt V. Pharmacogenetics of opioid receptors and addiction. Pharmacogenet Genomics 2006;16(1):1–7.
258.Ikeda K, Ogai Y, Nishizawa D, et al. Genetic analyses of morphine sensitivity. Nihon Rinsho 2005;63 Suppl 12:463–466.
259.Lotsch J, Geisslinger G. Relevance of frequent mu-opioid receptor polymorphisms for opioid activity in healthy volunteers. Pharmacogenomics J 2006;6(3):200–210.
260.Kim H, Neubert JK, San Miguel A, et al. Genetic influence on variability in human acute experimental pain sensitivity associated with gender, ethnicity and psychological temperament. Pain2004;109(3):488–496.
261.Caterina MJ, Leffler A, Malmberg AB, et al. Impaired nociception and pain sensation in mice lacking the capsaicin receptor. Science 2000;288(5464):306–313.
262.McKemy DD, Neuhausser WM, Julius D. Identification of a cold receptor reveals a general role for TRP channels in thermosensation. Nature 2002;416(6876):52–58.
263.Mogil JS, Ritchie J, Smith SB, et al. Melanocortin-1 receptor gene variants affect pain and mu-opioid analgesia in mice and humans. J Med Genet 2005;42(7):583–587.
264.Kest B, Hopkins E, Palmese CA, et al. Genetic variation in morphine analgesic tolerance: a survey of 11 inbred mouse strains. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2002;73(4):821–828.
265.Kest B, Palmese CA, Hopkins E, et al. Naloxone-precipitated withdrawal jumping in 11 inbred mouse strains: evidence for common genetic mechanisms in acute and chronic morphine physical dependence. Neuroscience 2002;115(2):463–469.
266.Kest B, Palmese CA, Juni A, et al. Mapping of a quantitative trait locus for morphine withdrawal severity. Mamm Genome 2004;15(8):610–617.
267.Liang DY, Liao G, Lighthall GK, et al. Genetic variants of the P-glycoprotein gene Abcb1b modulate opioid-induced hyperalgesia, tolerance and dependence. Pharmacogenet Genomics2006;16(11):825–835.
268.Liang DY, Guo T, Liao G, et al. Chronic pain and genetic background interact and influence opioid analgesia, tolerance, and physical dependence. Pain 2006;121(3):232–240.
269.Rathmell JP. American Society of Regional Anesthesia and Pain Medicine 2011 John J. Bonica Award Lecture: the evolution of the field of pain medicine. Reg Anesth Pain Med2012;37(6):652–656.
270.Lippe PM, Brock C, David J, et al. The First National Pain Medicine Summit—final summary report. Pain Med 2010;11(10): 1447–1468.
271.Cruciani RA. Treatment of pain with opioids and the risk for opioid dependence: the search for a balance. Adv Pain Man 2008; 2(1):2–3.